
A. M. PROVERBIO ET AL.
934
of presentation, and ethnic group of face. In a similar study,
where neutral and affective faces were bilaterally presented, it
was found a greater d’ in response to targets preceded by baby
than adult faces (Brosch et al., 2008), associated with an in-
crease in the P1 amplitude of ERPs recorded to valid trials. The
results were interpreted in terms of an early sensory modulation
due to attentional capturing mechanism for biologically rele-
vant stimuli, similar for baby faces and aversive stimuli (fearful
faces).
Our data showed no effect whatsoever of the ethnic group of
the faces, nor per se, nor in interaction with age face. RTs were
virtually identical for faces of all ethnic groups (Caucasian=
504.3 ms, other groups 504.4 ms) thus suggesting a complete
lack of ORE effect, not only in the attentional capabilities of
baby faces, but also in the spatial validity effects recorded for
adult faces. It must be considered that the ORE effect has been
interpreted in the literature has strongly linked to a difference in
the perceptual familiarity of own vs. other races, especially in
categorization task, based on featural analysis (Caldara et al.,
2004; Rhodes et al., 2009; Vizioli et al., 2011; Walker & Ta-
naka, 2003). In the case of the present investigation, no analysis
of face properties was required. Furthermore, a detailed face
featural analysis was made more difficult by the short presenta-
tion time (200 ms) and by the immediate presentation of the
target to be responded to. Therefore the baby face advantage
has to be considered an automatic and prioritarized response of
the visual system to biologically relevant signals. And indeed a
recent ERP study (Proverbio et al., 2011) provided evidence of
an increased visual N170 response to infant than adult faces
(especially in the female brain), thus supporting the previous
literature about a female preference for the image of infants
below 24 months (Proverbio et al., 2006).
At this regard, it cannot be excluded that some previous data
showing an effect of “race” on the response to baby faces might
be partly due to the specific faces used in that study as stimuli,
and their scarce number (i.e., only 8 “other race” and 8 “own
race” baby faces were used in the study by Hodsall and co-
workers (Hodsoll et al., 2010).
In our study, RTs were much faster in response to upright
than inverted trees, which is a rather common finding in cogni-
tive psychology literature (Shepard & Metzler, 1971), and has
to do with object familiarity, mental rotation, and same/differ-
ent effects (Logan, 1980). Target orientation also interacted
with cue validity, possibly because the “different” response
(“no”, with the middle finger), negatively interacted, at higher
order cognitive level, with the spatially-dependent attentional
advantage for the cued location, thus requiring the additional,
time-consuming, involvement of control systems, (such as, for
example, the anterior cingulate and other prefrontal structures
involved in conflict resolution, response monitoring and action
regulation), but further investigation is certainly needed to get
more light on this matter.
Conclusion
In summary, this study provides no evidence that faces (nei-
ther infantile nor adult) are processed differently as a function
of their ethnic group, (also called “race”, in some literature),
when it comes to automatic attentional orienting. However, a
powerful bias toward baby faces was found, with stronger cue-
ing validity effects for baby than adult faces, thus further sug-
gesting the existence of a fundamental social instinct that may
be at the basis of human offspring caregiving. Other neuroi-
maging data, regarding the preferential auditory processing, in
adults (and especially in women), of infant vocalizations such
as laughing and crying, do support this hypothesis (Sander et al.,
2007; Seifritz et al., 2003).
Acknowledgements
The study was funded by FAR2011 grant from University of
Milano Bicocca.
References
Brosch, T., Sander, D., Pourtois, G., & Scherer, K. R. (2008). Beyond
fear. Psychological Science, 19, 362-370.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02094.x
Brosch, T., Sander, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2007). That baby caught my
eye. Attention capture by infant faces. Emotion, 7, 685-689.
doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.685
Caldara, R., Rossion, B., Bovet, P., & Hauert, C.-A. (2004). Event-
related potentials and time course of the “other-race” face classifica-
tion advantage. NeuroReport, 15, 905-910.
doi:10.1097/00001756-200404090-00034
Glocker, M. L., Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J. W., Gur, R.
C., & Sachser, N. (2009). Baby schema in infant faces induces cute-
ness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology,
115, 257-263. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01603.x
Glocker, M. L., Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J. W., Val-
dez, J. N., Griffin, M. D., Sachser, N., & Gur, R. C. (2009). Baby
schema modulates the brain reward system in nulliparous women.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 9115-9119.
doi:10.1073/pnas.0811620106
Hodsoll, J., Quinn, K. A., & Hodsoll, S. (2010). Attentional prioritiza-
tion of infant faces is limited to own-race infants. PLoS ONE, 5,
e12509. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0012509
Kringelbach, M. L., Leh tonen, A., Sq uire, S., H arvey, A . G., Craske, M.
G., Holliday, I. E., Green, A. L., Aziz, T. Z., Hansen, P. C., Cor-
nelissen, P. L., & Stein, A. (2008). A specific and rapid neural sig-
nature for parental instinct. PLoS ONE, 3, e1664.
Leibenluft, E., Gobbini, M., Harrison, T., & Haxby, J. (2004). Mothers’
neural activation in response to picture of their children and other
children. Biolo g ic a l Psychiatry, 56, 225-232.
doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.05.017
Logan, G. D. (1980). Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming
tasks: Theory and data. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 523.
Lorenz, K. (1971). Studies in animal and human behavior. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(80)90019-5
Luo, L. Z., Li, H., & Lee, K. (2011). Are children’s faces really more
appealing than those of adults? Testing the baby schema hypothesis
beyond infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110,
115-124.
Mulckhuyse, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2010): Unconscious attentional ori-
enting to exogenous cues: A review of the literature. Acta Psy-
chologica, 134, 299-309. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.002
Nitschke, J., Nelson, E., Rusch, B., Fox, A., Oakes, T., & Davidson, R.
(2004). Orbitofrontal cortex tracks positive mood in mothers viewing
pictures of their newborn infants. Neuroimage, 21, 583-592.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.10.005
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. The Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 32, 3-25.
doi:10.1080/00335558008248231
Pourtois, G., Thut, G., De Peralta, R. G., Michel, C., & Vuilleumier, P.
(2005). Two electrophysiological stages of spatial orienting towards
fearful faces: Early temporo-parietal activation preceding gain con-
trol in extrastriate visual cortex. NeuroImage, 26, 149-163.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.015
Proverbio, A. M., Brignone, V., Matarazzo, S., Del Zotto, M., & Zani,
A. (2006). Gender differences in hemispheric asymmetry for face
processing. BMC Neuroscience, 8, 44.
doi:10.1186/1471-2202-7-44
Proverbio, A. M., Riva, F., Zani, A., & Martin, E. (2011). Is it a baby?
Perceived age affects brain processing of faces differently in women