
C. RASOAL ET AL.
928
the CFA showed that a two-factor model had an inadequate fit
with the data. There are two possible reasons why a two-factor
model did not fit the data. First, the two measures do not seem
to tap two distinct constructs, which is contrary to what Wang
et al. (2003) claimed. Second, our data set included a rather
homogenous sample, with only students from one university
and secondary schools. However, the CFA, together with the
high correlation between the two scales and the results of the
regression analyses, support the notion that the two scales
measure basically the same variable or share variance with a
third, common empathy construct. Overall, this was not in line
with our hypotheses. To conclude, basic empathy and ethno-
cultural empathy are highly interdependent. However, more
research is needed in this area before we will know how stable
the relationship between the two constructs is. Thus, at this
point, we do not claim that it is possible to translate basic em-
pathy to community empathy at a collective level.
This study raises questions regarding how ethnocultural em-
pathy should be measured. In particular, in light of the signifi-
cant association between impression management and empathy,
it might be that more indirect and implicit measures of empathy
should be used (e.g., Batson, Polycarpou, Harmon-Jones, Im-
hoff, & Mitchener, 1997). On the other hand, more experimen-
tal set-ups are hardly possible to administer in large samples
and therefore we believe that our self-reported findings add to
the literature on ethnocultural empathy. From a theoretical
point of view, we found no support for the contact hypothesis
that experience of ethnic diversity in a school does not predict
empathy ratings.
Limitations of the Study
There are two main limitations of this study. First we had a
relatively homogenous sample in a university and a secondary
school setting, with most participants being ethnic Swedes.
Future research could incorporate a wider range of participants
with respect to ethnicity and education. Second, we relied on
self-reporting measures that were translated from English into
Swedish. It could be that measures of empathy and, in particu-
lar ethnocultural empathy, do not translate easily to other set-
tings. On the other hand, our measures were generally well
understood and, as Sweden is a multicultural society with a
significant minority of immigrants (20%), it made sense to
measure ethnocultural empathy.
References
Alligood, M. R. (2007). Rethinking empathy in nursing education:
Shifting to a developmental view. In S. Leibold, & F. Maureen,
(Eds.), Middle range theory development using King’s conceptual
system (pp.287-296). New York, NY: Springer Publishing Co.
Alterman, A. I., McDermott, P. A., Cacciola, J. S., & Rutherford M. J.
(2003). Latent structure of the Davis interpersonal reactivity index in
methadone maintenance patients. Journal of Psychopathology and
Behavioral Assessment , 25, 257-265. doi:10.1023/A:1025936213110
Batson, C. D., Polycarpou, M. P., Harmon-Jones, E., Imhoff, H. J., &
Mitchener, E. C. (1997). Empathy and attitudes: Can feeling for a
member of a stigmatized group improve feelings toward the group?
Journal of Personality and Social psychology, 72, 105-118.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.1.105
Bentler, P. M. (2006). EQS 6 structural equations program manual.
Encino: Multivariate Software, Inc.
Cunidiff, L. N., & Komarraju, M. (2008). Gender differences in eth-
nocultural empathy and attitudes toward men and women in authority.
Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 15, 5-15.
doi:10.1177/1548051808318000
D’Ambrosio, F., Olivier, M., Didon, D., & Besche, C. (2009). The
basic empathy scale: A French validation of a measure of empathy in
youth. Persona lity and Individual Di fferences, 46, 160-165.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.09.020
Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy:
Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 44, 113-126.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113
Davis, M. H. (1996). Empathy: A social psychological approach.
Madison, WI: Brown & Benchmark.
De Corte, K., Buysse, A., Verhofstadt, L. L., Roeyers, H., Ponnet, K.,
& Davis, M. H. (2007). Measuring empathic tendencies: Reliability
and validity of the Dutch version of the interpersonal reactivity index.
Psychologica Belgica, 47, 235-260.
DiLalla, L. F., Hull, S. K., & Dorsey, J. K. (2004). Effect of gender, age,
and relevant course work on attitudes toward empathy, patient spiri-
tuality, and physician wellness. Teaching and Learning in Medicine,
16, 165-170. doi:10.1207/s15328015tlm1602_8
Duan, C., & Hill, C. (1996). The current state of empathy research.
Journal of Counselling Ps ych olo gy, 3, 261-274.
doi:10.1037/0022-0167.43.3.261
Dyche, L., & Zayas, L. H. (2001). Cross-cultural empathy and training
the contemporary psychotherapist. Clinical Social Work Journal, 29,
245-258. doi:10.1023/A:1010407728614
Eisenberg, N., & Lennon, R. (1983). Sex differences in empathy and
related capacities. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 100-131.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.94.1.100
Enberg, J., Kälvemark, S., & Ohlander, A.-S. (1998). The best genera-
tion. Falun: AiT Scandbook.
Endresen, I. M., & Olweus, D. (2001) Self-reported empathy in Nor-
wegian adolescents: Sex differences, age trends, and relationship to
bullying. In A. C. Bohart, & D. J. Stipek (Eds.), Constructive & de-
structive behavior: Implications for family, school, & society (pp.
147-165). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Hojat, M., Mangione, S., Kane, G. C., & Gonnella, J. S. (2005).
Relationships between scores of the Jefferson scale of physician
empathy (JSPE) and the interpersonal reactivity index (IRI). Medical
Teacher, 27, 625-628. doi:10.1080/01421590500069744
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in co-
variance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alterna-
tives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1, 55.
Kim, S. S., Kaplowitz, S., & Johnston, M.V. (2004). The effects of
physician empathy on patient satisfaction and compliance. Journal of
Evaluation and the Health Professi ons, 27, 237-251.
doi:10.1177/0163278704267037
Mercer, S.W., & Reynolds, W. J. (2002). Empathy and quality of care.
The British Journal of General Practice, 5 2, 9-12.
Pastor, A. R. (2004). Differences in empathy in gender and age.
Apuntes de Psicologia, 22, 323-339.
Paulhus, D. L. (1988). Assessing self-deception and impression man-
agement in self-report: The balanced inventory of desirable respond-
ing. Vancouver: Department of Psychology, University of British
Colombia.
Pettigrew, T. F. (1998). Intergroup contact theory. Annual Review of
Psychology, 49, 65-85. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.65
Rasoal, C., Jungert, T., Hau, S., & Andersson, A. (Accepted). Devel-
opment of a Swedish version of the Scale of Ethnocultural Empathy.
Psychology, 2, 568-573.
Rasoal, C., Eklund, J., & Hansen, E. (2011). Toward conceptualization
of ethnocultural empathy. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cul-
tural Psychology, 5, 1-13.
Rasoal, C., Jungert, T., Hau, S., Edvardsson-Stiwne, E., & Andersson,
A. (2009). Ethnocultural and basic empathy among students in health
education. Evaluation & the Health Profession, 3, 300-313.
doi:10.1177/0163278709338569
Ridley, C. R., & Lingle, D. W. (1996). Cultural empathy in multicul-
tural counselling: A multidimensional process model. In P. B.
Pedersen, & J. G. Draguns (Eds.), Counsel li n g ac ross culture (4th ed.,
pp. 21-46). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Schieman, S., & Van Gundy, K. V. (2000). The personal and social
links between age and self-reported empathy. Social Psychology
Quarterly, 63, 152-174. doi:10.2307/2695889