 International Journal of Intelligence Science, 2011, 1, 17-24 doi:10.4236/ijis.2011.12003 Published Online October 2011 (http://www.SciRP.org/journal/ijis) Copyright © 2011 SciRes. IJIS Dissociating Improvement of Attention and Intelligence during Written Language Acquisition in Adults Steffen Landgraf1,2,3, Reinhard Beyer1, Ann Pannekamp1, Gesa Schaadt1, Darina Koch1, Manja Foth1, Elke van der Meer1,2 1Humboldt-Un iversität zu Berlin, Institute of Psychology, Berlin, Germany 2School of Mind and Brain, Berli n, Germany 3Sorbonne University, Paris, France E-mail: S.Landgraf@HU-Berlin.de Received July 28, 2011; revised September 22, 2011; accepted September 30, 2011 Abstract About one tenth of the world’s population cannot read and write sufficiently. Cognitive abilities, such as se- lective attention and crystallized as well as fluid intelligence, have been defined as crucial factors for the ac- quisition of written language skills. However, it is unclear whether these abilities are necessary also for the alphabetization of adults. Before and after a one-year alphabetization course, we compared the attention and intelligence of 47 illiterate individuals to 41 matched literate controls who did not take part in the alphabeti- zation course. Illiterate individuals improved in selective attention and crystallized intelligence from before to after the alphabetization course; however, they did not reach the same level of functioning as literate con- trols. In addition, the fluid intelligence of illiterates did not improve. More importantly, when controlling for attention improvement, we found that improvement in crystallized intelligence was associated with alpha- betization above and beyond the influence of attention. Our results suggest that alphabetization is closely related to improvements in attention and crystallized intelligence. Specifically, socio-cultural, knowledge- specific learning processes improve during the acquisition of written language skills and may not depend on only the enhancement of the ability to attend to relevant stimuli. Alphabetization programs may, therefore, benefit from distinct considerations of attentional, intellectual, and literacy related skill acquisitions. Keywords: Alphabetization Course, Illiterate Adults, Crystallized Intelligence, Fluid Intelligence, Attention, Written Language Acquisition 1. Introduction This One challenge of modern societies is to educate their members in reading and writing, the so called al- phabetization. Worldwide, there are about 796 million illiterate individuals, two thirds of whom are women [1]. Even in highly developed countries such as Germany, 7.5 million individuals cannot read or write proficiently [2]. Hence, understanding and improving the effective- ness of alphabetization programs is of great interest. Whereas there have been multiple investigations of cog- nitive abilities and their association with literacy, rather little is known about the cognitive processes underlying actual alphabetization. The present study investigates precisely this issue, looking at specific cognitive abilities and their contribution to alphabetization in illiterate adults. Cognitive abilities, such as phonological awareness, attention, and intelligence, have been proposed to be associated with written language acquisition (for a dis- cussion on phonological awareness, please refer to [3]). Baddeley’s working memory model [4,5] assigns a pri- mordial role to a “central executive” regarding the ability to attend to relevant and to inhibit irrelevant information. Attention and intelligence may be crucial for written language acquisition because they are of utmost impor- tance during learning [6-9]. In fact, intelligence and at- tention relate to alphabetization as they allow effective investment of cognitive resources during educational processes [10,11]. Regarding literacy, a plethora of studies has shown that it is robustly associated with central executive func-
 S. LANDGRAF ET AL. 18 tioning. For example, verbal span of preschoolers can predict future written language skills [12]. Further, working memory span in fourth to sixth graders is asso- ciated with the ability to read, specifically with faster reading speed [13]. Interestingly, the interrelation be- tween central executive and written language skills (e.g., decoding speed) increases with increasing reading abili- ties: good readers show stronger associations than poor readers, indicating that a lack of exposure to written lan- guage leads to a weak automatization of relevant cogni- tive processes [14,15]. Consequently, for individuals with weakly developed reading and writing skills, under- standing written language requires a greater investment of cognitive abilities, which results, in turn, in impaired performance. Individuals with poor central executive abilities show also less flexible selective attention [16]. Reference [17] points out that decreased selective attention reduces al- phabetization success, supporting the assumption that attention plays a decisive role in written language acqui- sition. Yet its interrelation with other cognitive abilities during written language acquisition in adults is rather unknown. With the present study, we investigated whe- ther the influence of alphabetization on selective atten- tion could be dissociated from the influence on other cognitive abilities associated with the central executive, namely, intelligence. Intelligence is strongly associated with the allocation of cognitive resources [18] and may facilitate the acqui- sition and maintenance of written language skills. Ac- cording to the general intelligence model of Cattel [19], there are two intelligence factors: fluid and crystallized. Fluid intelligence describes the capacity to find solutions for complex problems in new situations—a decisive fac- tor for learning processes [8,20-22]. According to the investment theory [23], problem solving, reasoning, or classifying allow an efficient adaptation to new situations in which stable representations of new knowledge are formed [24]. Knowledge acquisition correlates positively with fluid intelligence level [6-9], especially if the learn- ing process is highly complex and innovative [8,20,21, 25]. During alphabetization the learner is confronted with new and complex situations, implying a complex learning pattern [26]. For example, rhyme detection [27, 28] and syllable discrimination [29,30] have already been developed before entering primary school. These abilities are relevant not only for structuring written lan- guage, but they are important also for substituting pre- fixes and suffixes in order to change word meanings [31]. Fluid intelligence may support these processes and, con- sequently, it may support the acquisition of written lan- guage. Nevertheless, socio-cultural factors contributing to the manifestation of content-specific knowledge may affect alphabetization even more specifically. Crystallized intelligence accounts for the capacity to accumulate knowledge based on socio-cultural and edu- cational background [19,23,32]. It is influenced, though not exclusively, by fluid intelligence and environment- specific learning conditions such as, for example, paren- tal socio-economic status [33]. Crystallized knowledge can be applied to solve content-specific problems [34, 35]. For example, while non-alphabetic, idiosyncratic symbols, such as Arabic numbers, do not represent pho- netic or graphemic units, they reflect pronounceable words [36]. Illiterates are able to acquire the Arabic number system informally [37] and can even conduct various, though simple, calculations [10,38,39]. The pro- ficiency in manipulating numbers indicates that symbol systems associated with simple written language de- mands can be acquired by illiterates, even without formal education. This form of learning, which is strongly de- pendent on crystallized intelligence, influences also the alphabetization process. In the present study, the main question was whether the influence of alphabetization on attention and intelli- gence could be dissociated. We assessed selective atten- tion as well as fluid and crystallized intelligence of illit- erate adults before and after a one-year alphabetization course. We hypothesized that specifically crystallized intelligence scores would improve during the alphabeti- zation course. Further, in order to dissociate attention and intelligence improvement during written language acquisition, we subdivided illiterates into two groups: those who improved significantly in selective attention during the alphabetization course and those who did not. We expected improvements in attention to affect im- provements in intelligence if attentional abilities were the driving factors of alphabetization. Finally, we compared attention and intelligence between illiterate and literate individuals. 2. Material and Methods 2.1. Partic ipa n ts Literate controls (n = 41) were matched to illiterate indi- viduals (n = 47; Table 1). All illiterate individuals and three literate controls were of non-German origin who have been living in Germany for an average of 11.5 years (standard deviation, SD = 7.8 y) at the time of testing. Literate controls were carefully selected regarding edu- cation duration. The study followed guidelines in accor- dance with the Declaration of Helsinki (1964). All par- ticipants were paid and provided consent before inclu- sion. Copyright © 2011 SciRes. IJIS
 S. LANDGRAF ET AL.19 Table 1. Demographic information for the two participant groups. Illiterate group Literate group N 47 41 Gender 29F 21F Age 38.3 (8.8) 34.0 (11.3) Years of education 4.4 (3.9) 11.2 (1.4) Handedness 44R, 3L 39R, 1L, 1M Course attendance (days) 132 (34) - Note: N = number of participants; F = females; years of education = number of years individuals spent in regular school (12 = university entrance certi- fication); R = right-handed; L = left-handed; M = mixed-handed; course attendance = length of time illiterates took part in the alphabetization course; numbers in parentheses = standard deviations. 2.2. Materials 2.2.1. Procedure To evaluate the improvement of abilities associated with alphabetization, the illiterate group was tested before being included in the course (T1) and after having been trained (one year later, T2). The alphabetization course entailed weekly sessions where participants were pre- sented with letters, words, and short phrases from dif- ferent topics relevant to their daily lives (e.g., buying groceries). In addition to visual and auditory material for each new letter, participants received feedback each week about their performance. With increasing compe- tence level, participants were offered the possibility to self-paced e-learning. There was only one testing session for the literate con- trols since they did not participate in the alphabetization course. Participants were tested in groups of ten. The order of tests was always in the order in which they are described below. Due to technical difficulties, the data from the d2 test for two illiterate individuals and the data from the CFT-20-R for one illiterate individual were excluded from analyses. 2.2.2. Stimuli The d2 test of attention. The d2 test [40] is a language- independent test that evaluates selective attention. The test requires participants to detect and cross out targets (letter d with two dashes) as quickly and accurately as possible while ignoring distracters (e.g., letter d with one or three dashes). The test consists of 14 rows, each con- taining 47 signs. The time limit for each row is 20 s. We report a measure for attention calculated from the quality, quantity, and time of the d2 performance. The test takes 8 minutes. CFT-20-R. The CFT-20-R is a language-free test as- sessing fluid intelligence [19] independently of culturally specific knowledge [41]. The first part of the CFT-20-R consists of four subtests, namely, sequence completion, classification, matrices, and topology. The tasks of each subtest are in multiple-choice formats and are ordered from easiest to most difficult. The number of correct responses allows for the calculation of a fluid intelli- gence score. The test takes 14 minutes. CFT-ZF-R. The CFT-ZF-R is also a language-free test assessing crystallized intelligence [19] by testing nu- merical processing capacity [41]. It consists of 21 multi- ple-choice tasks in which number sequences have to be continued in a logical way. The number of correct re- sponses allows for the calculation of a crystallized intel- ligence score. The test takes 12 minutes. 2.2.3. Data Analysis PASW 18 (Predictive Analysis SoftWare) was used to perform statistical analyses. Data were, unless otherwise specified, normally distributed (Kolgomorov-Smirnov test). The significance level for all statistical tests was 0.05. For all tests, the raw data were converted into stan- dardized values representing percentile ranks. A percen- tile rank of 25%, for example, implies that the individual performed better than 25% and worse than 75% of his/ her age group. To investigate whether the illiterate group improved on the experimental measures, we used paired t-tests with time of testing (before the alphabetization or T1 vs. after the alphabetization course or T2) as a within-sub- jects factor for the illiterate group. Further, the test re- sults were compared between groups (illiterate vs. liter- ate) using independent t-tests separately for the first (T1) and the second (T2) testing sessions of the illiterate group. To dissociate the influence of attention and intelli- gence on written language acquisition, we subdivided the illiterate group based on their improvement on the d2 test: high d2 improvement and low d2 improvement from T1 to T2. We investigated whether both subgroups different- tially improved on the other tests. In other words, did individuals who improved highly (weakly) on the d2 also improve highly (weakly) on the other tests? There were two approaches to test this. First, we compared test per- formances between times of testing (T1 vs. T2) using paired t-tests individually for the two subgroups. Second, we calculated an improvement measure for each test as the difference between test performance at T2 minus test performance at T1. We then used independent sample t-tests to compare improvements between subgroups (illiterates who showed low vs. high d2 improvement) in each test. Copyright © 2011 SciRes. IJIS
 S. LANDGRAF ET AL. 20 3. Results 3.1. Demographic Variables The literate and illiterate groups did not differ in gender, 2 1 = 0.98, p > 0.05), age (t(86) = −2.03, p > 0.05, or handedness, 2 2 =1.90, p > 0.05, but did differ in years of education, t(59) = −11.12, p < 0.01; see Table 1. 3.2. d2 Test of Attention Participants in the illiterate group improved in d2 per- formance from T1 to T2, t(44) = −5.71, p < 0.01. Com- pared to the literate group, participants in the illiterate group performed worse at T1, t(57.35) = −8.62, p < 0.01, and at T2, t(83) = −3.98, p < 0.01; see Table 2. 3.3. CFT-20-R Participants in the illiterate group did not improve in their fluid intelligence scores from T1 to T2, t(45) = −1.79, p > 0.05. Illiterate individuals showed worse per- formance at T1, t(66.99) = −11.31, p < 0.01, and T2, t(85) = −9.67, p < 0.02, compared to the literate group. 3.4. CFT-ZF-R Participants in the illiterate group improved in their crystallized intelligence scores from T1 to T2, t(46) = −2.69, p < 0.01. Compared to the literate group, the illit- erate group’s performance was worse at T1, t(43.99) = −9.37, p < 0.01, and T2, t(67.35) = −7.11, p < 0.01. 3.5. Controlling for Attention According to the d2 improvement of the illiterate group, we differentiated between a high-improvement subgroup (HI, mean d2 improvement = 41.9 (±18.6) percentile ranks, n = 17) and a low-improvement subgroup (LI, Table 2. Results of the cognitive tests for the illiterate group (two assessment s) and the literate group (one assessment). Illit erate gr ou p Before the alphabetization After the alphabetization Literate group d2 7.2 (15) 25.0 (30) 49.9 (28) CFT-20-R 7.4 (18) 11.1 (24) 63.6 (27) CFT-ZF-R 2.0 (7) 8.0 (20) 47.4 (30) Note: All values are given in percentile rank according to the tests’ manuals. On all tasks and at all times, the illiterate group performed worse than the literate group. Numbers in bold for the illiterate group indicate improve- ments in percentile rank from before to after the alphabetization class. mean d2 improvement = 0.8 (±1.2) percentile ranks, n = 15). This enabled us to control for the influence of atten- tion improvement regarding the improvement of intelli- gence during written language acquisition without rely- ing on, e.g., correlational analyses. Regarding the CFT- 20-R (fluid intelligence), neither subgroup improved from T1 to T2 (HI-group: t(14) = 1.58, p > 0.05; LI-group: t(15) = 1.51, p > 0.05). Further, the CFT-20-R improvement score (score at T2 minus score at T1) did not differ between subgroups, t(29) = −1.45, p > 0.05. Regarding the CFT-ZF-R (crystallized intelligence), the subgroup improving strongly in d2 scores showed a trend toward improvement from T1 to T2 (HI-group: t(14) = −1.83, p = 0.09; LI-group: t(15) = −1.45, p > 0.05). The subgroups did not differ in CFT-ZF-R improvement, t(29) = −1.80, p > 0.05. Together, these results suggest that selective attention improvement during the alphabetiza- tion course is mostly unrelated to the improvement in intelligence during the same period (see Figure 1). 4. Discussion The present study investigated how attention and intelli- gence are affected by alphabetization in illiterate adults. Whereas crystallized intelligence and selective attention improved from before to after the alphabetization course, fluid intelligence did not. When controlling for attention improvements, the improvement patterns of intelligence during the alphabetization course did not change: crys- tallized intelligence improved regardless of attention; fluid intelligence did not improve regardless of attention. Despite showing enhanced selective attention and crys- tallized intelligence at the end of the alphabetization course, illiterate individuals did not reach the perform- ance levels of literate individuals. We conclude that im- provements of selective attention during alphabetization do not necessarily influence intelligence improvements. Thus, attention and intelligence measures may make unique contributions to the acquisition of written lan- guage skills. According to our main hypotheses, during alphabeti- zation adult illiterates selectively improve in their cogni- tive abilities. Attention and crystallized intelligence im- provements paralleled the administration of a one-year alphabetization course. This is in line with studies show- ing that better attention and intelligence scores are asso- ciated with higher alphabetization [17,31,37]. Further- more, crystallized intelligence can be improved as a function of available knowledge and intellectual de- mands [42-44]. Due to developing literacy, there is an acquisition of new knowledge, which can be applied to new situations. As a consequence, previously inaccessi- ble information can be accessed and, in turn, serves as a Copyright © 2011 SciRes. IJIS
 S. LANDGRAF ET AL. Copyright © 2011 SciRes. IJIS 21 D2 Hi gh Im pr ovement 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 CFT-20-R CFT-ZF-R Percentile Rank T1 T2 (a) D2 Low Impr ovem ent 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 CFT-20-R CFT-ZF-R Percentile Rank T1 T2 (b) Figure 1. (a) Intelligence test perfor mance (CFT-20-R and CFT-ZF-R) of the d2 high-improving group (HI) at T1 (before the alphabetization course) and T2 (after the alphabetization course); (b) Intelligence test performance (CFT-20-R and CFT-ZF-R) of the d2 low-improving group (LI) at T1 (before the alphabetization course) and T2 (after the alphabetization course). Note: in the D2 high-improving group, variability of the percentile rank was more pronounced compared to the D2 low -improvi ng g ro up. Thi s may hav e be en du e t o the f act th at the maj ori t y of i ndi vidual s i n t h e D 2 l ow - i mprovi ng g rou p ha d zero improvement; in other words a floor eff ect. foundation for providing new knowledge input and new knowledge manifestation. More importantly, our results showed that the process of “crystallizing” new knowledge appears to be possible even above and beyond the influence of attentional func- tions. We dissociated the improvement of crystallized intelligence and selective attention. Specifically, we con- trolled for attention by subdividing illiterate individuals according to their attentional improvement score. This method has, to our knowledge, not been used before. By explicitly not relying on correlational analyses, which are typically used in the literature, we demonstrate that crys- tallized intelligence increased regardless of whether at- tention improved or not. This specifies former research
 S. LANDGRAF ET AL. 22 showing that automatization of cognitive processes in- creases with better written language abilities [14,15]. Specifically, exposure to written language requires illit- erates to allocate more cognitive resources and, therefore, to extensively practice intellectual abilities during al- phabetization programs. According to our results, this allocation of cognitive resources may be independent of attention training effects. Fluid intelligence measures did not improve and were also not influenced by attention improvement during alphabetization. According to Cattell [19], fluid intelli- gence is a decisive factor when learning to solve [8,21, 22] and when actually solving complex problems [20, 25]. The results of the present study suggest that alpha- betization does not reflect improvements in fluid intelli- gence. This may be due to the fact that in order to ac- quire written language skills, children rely on cognitive abilities that were acquired before entering primary school [26-29] and that may be culturally specific [45]. For example, [46,47] showed that children from the United Kingdom and from Zambia did not differ when reproducing abstract patterns with clay. However, British outperformed Zambian children when reproducing pat- terns with papers and pencils. By contrast, Zambian were better than British children when reproducing patterns with wires. Hence, the familiarity with material proper- ties can influence the development of cognitive abilities [48], implying that the ability to recognize rules or regu- larities and to draw inferences [49] can be supported by domain-specific learning processes and knowledge [33]. Instead of fluid intelligence, culture-specific expertise, that is, crystallized intelligence, may be the decisive fac- tor for written language acquisition success [50]. In line with former studies [51,52], our results confirm that illiterate individuals do not reach cognitive abilities as shown by literate controls. Indeed, in cognitive tests that are related to written language, illiterate individuals have demonstrated performance comparable to that of preschoolers [51]. In the present study, it can be assumed that the cognitive abilities of illiterate individuals were not trained as effectively as would have been necessary in order to reach the level of literate individuals. Before the alphabetization course, attention was below the lower norm. The decreased exposure to educationally relevant contents mediated by illiteracy may have impeded the development of cognitive functions [10,53-56]. Thus, the weak automatization of cognitive processes affects the effective interplay regarding not only written language skills, but also attention and intelligence. During adult- hood, this may increase difficulties when acquiring new knowledge, specifically written language skills. The present investigation has some limitations. The sample size was small and the duration of the alphabeti- zation course was relatively short. Future studies should look further into, first, how cognitive abilities can be improved. Second, given the difference in fluid intelli- gence between the literate and illiterate group in our sample, future investigations should investigate how il- literate individuals can possibly reach the level of literate individuals longitudinally. Third, the control group was only tested once whereas the illiteracy group was tested twice. Spontaneous learning might have taken place in the illiteracy group due to repeated test exposure. How- ever, there were more than 12 months between the two testing sessions making it unlikely to produce improve- ment effects. Moreover, there is no evidence for per- formance improvement due to multiple testing in the d2-test [40]. Fourth, as a function of illiterates catching up with literates, individual written language acquisition learning rates could be identified in order to characterize the postulated increase of automatization and interrela- tion between cognitive abilities. Consequently, the pre- dictive power of cognitive abilities, their interrelations, and individual improvements could be strengthened as a prerequisite for the optimization of alphabetization pro- grams. 5. Conclusions This study illuminated specific aspects of the interplay between attention, intelligence, and alphabetization. Se- lective attention and crystallized intelligence, but not fluid intelligence, of illiterate adults improved during a one-year alphabetization course. However, illiterates did not attain the same level of cognitive functioning as shown by literate controls. Interestingly, crystallized intelligence improved above and beyond the influence of attentional improvement. These results suggest, first, that alphabetization is closely related to improvements in attention and socio-cultural (i.e., knowledge-specific) learning processes. Second, these learning processes probably do not depend on only an increase in the ability to attend to relevant stimuli. Third, alphabetization suc- cess may be improved by taking into account attention, intelligence, and literacy related factors, separately. 6. Acknowledgements This research was supported by a grant from the German Federal Ministry for Education and Research (01AB 091002). 7. References [1] UNESCO, “Education for All. Global Monitoring Re- port,” Oxford University Press, Paris, 2010. Copyright © 2011 SciRes. IJIS
 S. LANDGRAF ET AL.23 [2] A. Grotlüschen and W. Riekmann, “Leo.—Level-One Studie. Literalität von Erwachsenen auf den Unteren Kompetenzniveaus,” Presseheft, 2011, pp. 1-13. [3] S. Landgraf, R. Beyer, I. Hild, N. Schneider, E. Horn, G. Schaadt, M. Foth, A. Pannekamp and E. van der Meer, “Impact of Phonological Processing Skills on Written Language Acquisition in Illiterate Adults,” Develop- mental Cognitive Neuroscien ce, in press. [4] A. D. Baddeley, N. Thomson and J. A. Buchanan, “Word Length and the Structure of Short-Term Memory,” Jour- nal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, Vol. 14, No. 6, 1975, pp. 575-589. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(75)80045-4 [5] A. D. Baddeley, “The Episodic Buffer: A New Compo- nent of Working Memory?” Trends in Cognitive Science, Vol. 4, No. 11, 2000, pp. 417-423. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2 [6] D. Z. Hambrick, J. E. Pink, E. J. Meinz, J. C. Pettibone and F. L. Oswald, “The Roles of Ability, Personality, and Interests in Acquiring Current Events Knowledge: A Longitudinal Study,” Intelligence, Vol. 36, No. 2, 2008, pp. 261-278. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2007.06.004 [7] E. Tamez, J. Myerson and S. Hale, “Learning, Working Memory, and Intelligence Revisited,” Behavioral Proc- esses, Vol. 78, No. 2, 2008, pp. 240-245. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2008.01.008 [8] M. W. Watkins, P. Lei and G. L. Canivaz, “Psychometric Intelligence and Achievement: A Cross-Lagged Panel Analysis,” Intelligence, Vol. 35, No. 1, 2007, pp. 59-68. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.04.005 [9] B. A. Williams and S. L. Pearlberg, “Learning of Three-Term Contingencies Correlates with Raven Scores, but Not with Measures of Cognitive Processing,” Intelli- gence, Vol. 34, No. 2, 2006, pp. 177-191. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2005.03.007 [10] M. Rosselli, A. Ardila and P. Rosas, “Neuropsychological Assessment in Illiterates. II. Language and Praxic Abili- ties,” Brain and Cognition, Vol. 12, No. 2, 1990, pp. 281-296. do i:10.1016/0278-2626(90)90020-O [11] S. W. Cornelius and A. Caspi, “Everyday Problem Solv- ing in Adulthood and Old Age,” Psychological Aging, Vol. 2, No. 2, 1987, pp. 144-153. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.2.2.144 [12] V. A. Mann and I. Y. Liberman, “Phonological Aware- ness and Verbal Short-Term Memory,” Journal of Learning Disabilities, Vol. 17, No. 10, 1984, pp. 592-599. doi:10.1177/002221948401701005 [13] P. de Jonge and P. F. de Jong, “Working Memory, Intel- ligence and Reading Ability in Children,” Personality and Individual Differences, Vol. 21, No. 6, 1996, pp. 1007-1020. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00161-4 [14] K. E. Stanovich, A. E. Cunningham and D. J. Feeman, “Intelligence, Cognitive Skills, and Early Reading Pro- gress,” Reading Research Quarterly, Vol. 19, No. 3, 1984, pp. 278-303. doi:10.2307/747822 [15] J. T. Guthrie, “Models of Reading and Reading Disabil- ity,” Journal of Educational Psychology, Vol. 65, No. 1, 1973, pp. 9-18. doi:10.1037/h0034799 [16] M. K. Bleckley, F. T. Durso, J. M. Crutchfield, R. W. Engle and M. M. Khanna, “Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity Predict Visual Attention Al- location,” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, Vol. 10, No. 4, 2003, pp. 884-889. doi:10.3758/BF03196548 [17] K. E. Stanovich and A. E. Cunningham, “What Reading Does for the Mind,” Journal of Direct Instructions, Vol. 1, No. 2, 2001, pp. 137-149. [18] E. van der Meer, et al., “Resource Allocation and Fluid Intelligence: Insights from Pupillometry,” Psychophysi- ology, Vol. 47, No. 1, 2010, pp. 158-169. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00884.x [19] R. B. Cattell, “Theory of Fluid and Chrystalized Intelli- gence: A Critical Experiment,” Journal of Educational Psychology, Vol. 54, No. 1, 1963, pp. 1-22. doi:10.1037/h0046743 [20] A. V. Kvist and J. Gustafsson, “The Relation between Fluid Intelligence and the General Factor as a Function of Cultural Background: A Test of Cattell’s Investment Theory,” Intelligence, Vol. 36, 2008, pp. 422-436. [21] M. C. Voelkle, W. W. Wittmann and P. L. Ackermann, “Abilities and Skill Acquisition: A Latent Growth Curve Approach,” Learning and Individual Differences, Vol. 16, No. 4, 2006, pp. 303-319. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2006.01.001 [22] K. S. McGrew, “CHC Theory and the Human Cognitive Abilities Project: Standing on the Shoulders of the Giants of Psychometric Intelligence Research,” Intelligence, Vol. 37, No. 1, 2009, pp. 1-22. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2008.08.004 [23] R. B. Cattell, “Abilities, Their Structure, Growth, and Action,” Houghton Mifflin, Boston, 1971. [24] J. J. McArdle, F. Hamagami, W. Meredith and K. P. Bradway, “Modeling the dynamic hypotheses of Gf–Gc theory using longitudinal life-span data,” Learning and Individual Differences, Vol. 12, 2000, pp. 53-79. doi:10.1016/S1041-6080(00)00036-4 [25] P. L. Ackerman, and A. T. Cianciolo, “Ability and Task Constraint Determinants of Complex Task Performance,” Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied, Vol. 8, No. 3, 2002, pp. 194-208. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.8.3.194 [26] C. D. Schnitzler, “Phonologische Bewusstheit und Schrifts- pracherwerb,” Thieme, Stuttgart, 2008. [27] J. C. Lenel and J. H. Cantor, “Rhyme Recognition and Phonemic Perception in Young Children,” Cognition, Vol. 10, No. 1, 1981, pp. 57-67. doi:10.1007/BF01067361 [28] L. Bradley and P. E. Bryant, “Categorizing Sounds and Learning to Read: A Causal Connection,” Cognition, Vol. 301, No. 5899, 1983, pp. 419-421. doi:10.1038/301419a0 [29] I. Y. Liberman, D. Shankweiler, F. W. Fisher and B. Carter, “Explicit Syllable and Phoneme Segmentation in the Young Child,” Cognition, Vol. 18, No. 2, 1974, pp. 201- 212. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(74)90101-5 [30] H. Jansen, G. Mannhaupt, H. Marx and H. Skowronek, “Bielefelder Screening zur Früherkennung von Lese- Copyright © 2011 SciRes. IJIS
 S. LANDGRAF ET AL. Copyright © 2011 SciRes. IJIS 24 Rechschreibschwierigkeiten (BISC),” Hogrefe, Göttingen, 2002. [31] H. S. Scaraborough, L. C. Ehri, R. K. Olson and A. E. Fowler, “The Fate of Phonemic Awareness beyond the Elementary School Years,” Scientific Studies of Reading, Vol. 2, No. 2, 1998, pp. 115-142. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0202_2 [32] J. Horn, “Measurement of Intellectual Capabilities: A Review of Theory,” In: K. S. McGrew, J. K. Werder and R. W. Woodcock, Eds., WJ-R Technical Manual, Riverside, Chicago, 1991. [33] H. Rindermann, C. Flores-Mendoza and M. Mansur-Alves, “Reciprocal Effects between Fluid and Crystallized Intelli- gence and Their Dependence on Parents’ Socioeconomic Status and Education,” Learning and Individual Differ- ences, Vol. 20, No. 5, 2010, pp. 544-548. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.07.002 [34] H. J. Markowitch and H. Welzer, “Das Autobiographische Gedächtnis,” Klett Cotta, Stuttgart, 2005. [35] K. W. Schaie, “Developmental Influences on Adult Intel- ligence: The Seattle longitudinal Study,” Oxford Univer- sity Press, New York, 2005. [36] K. Wilmes, “Störungen der Zahlenverarbeitung,” In: W. Hartje and K. Poeck, Eds., Klinische Neuropsychologie, de Gruyter, Berlin, 2006. [37] G. Wood, H. C. Nuerk, P. Freitas, G. Freitas and K. Willmes, “What Do Semi-Illiterate Adults Know about 2-Digit Arabic Numbers?” Cortex, Vol. 42, No. 1, 2006, pp. 48-56. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70321-6 [38] A. Castro-Caldas, A. Reis and M. Guerreiro, “Neuropsy- chological Aspects of Illiteracy,” Neuropsychological Rehabilitation; Vol. 7, No. 4, 1997, pp. 327-338. doi:10.1080/713755546 [39] G. Deloche, L. Souza, L. W. Braga and G. Dellatolas, “A Calculation and Number Processing Battery for Clinical Application in Illiterates and Semi-Literates,” Cortex, Vol. 35, No. 4, 1999, pp. 503-521. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70815-3 [40] R. Brickenkamp and E. Zillmer. “The d2 Test of Atten- tion,” Hogrefe & Huber, Göttingen, 2002. [41] R. H. Weiss, “Grundintelligenztest Skala 2,” Hogrefe, Göttingen, 2006. [42] P. B. Baltes, U. Lindenberger and U. M. Staudinger, “Die zwei Gesichter der Intelligenz im Alter. [Intelligence and Aging—Two Sides of the Coin],” Spektrum der Wissen- schaft, Vol. 10, 1995, pp. 52-61. [43] K. W. Schaie and S. L. Willis, “Age Difference Patterns of Psychometric Intelligence in Adulthood: Generalizability within and across Ability Domains,” Psychology & Aging, Vol. 8, No. 1, 1993, pp. 44-55. doi:10.1037//0882-7974.8.1.44 [44] U. M. Staudinger, J. Smith and P. B. Baltes, “Wis- dom-Related Knowledge in a Life Review Task: Age Differences and the Role of Professional Specialization,” Psycho log y & Aging, Vol. 7, No. 2, 1992, pp. 271-281. doi:10.1037//0882-7974.7.2.271 [45] F. Ostrosky-Solis, M. Ramirez and A. Ardila, “Effects of Culture and Education on Neuropsychological Testing: A Preliminary Study with Indigenous and Nonindigenous Population,” Applied Neuropsychology, Vol. 11, No. 4, 2004, pp. 188-195. [46] R. Serpell, “How Specific Are Perceptual Skills? A Cross-Cultural Study of Pattern Reproduction,” British Journal of Psychology, Vol. 70, No. 3, 1979, pp. 365-380. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1979.tb01706.x [47] R. Serpell, “The Cultural Construction of Intelligence,” In: W. J. Lonner and R. Halpas, Eds., Psychology and Culture, Allyn & Bacon, Boston, 1994, pp. 157-163. [48] D. Miller-Jones, “Culture and Testing,” American Psy- chology, Vol. 44, No. 2, 1989, pp. 360-366. [49] K. J. Klauer and G. D. Phye, “Inductive Reasoning: A Training Approach,” Review of Educational Research, Vol. 78, No. 1, 2008, pp. 85-123. doi:10.3102/0034654307313402 [50] K. A. Ericsson, R. T. Krampe and C. Tesch-Römer, “The Role of Deliberate Practice in the Acquisition of Expert Performance,” Psychological Review, Vol. 100, No. 3, 1993, pp. 363-406. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.100.3.363 [51] G. Dellatolas, et al., “Cognitive Consequences of Early Phase of Literacy,” Journal of the International Neuro- psychological Society, Vol. 9, No. 5, 2003, pp. 771-782. do i:10.1017/S1355617703950107 [52] J. J. Manly, et al., “Effect of Literacy on Neuropsy- chological Test Performance in Nondemented, Educa- tion-Matched Elders,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, Vol. 5, No. 3, 1999, pp. 191- 202. do i:10.1017/S135561779953302X [53] A. Ardila, M. Rosselli and P. Rosas, “Neuropsychologi- cal Assessment in Illiterates: Visuospatial and Memory Abilities,” Brain and Cognition, Vol. 11, No. 2, 1989, pp. 147-166. doi:10.1016/0278-2626(89)90015-8 [54] A. R. Lecours, et al., “Illiteracy and Brain Damage. 2. Manifestations of Unilateral Neglect in Testing “Auditory Comprehension” with Iconographic Materials,” Brain and Cognition, Vol. 6, No. 3, 1987, pp. 243-265. do i:10.1016/0278-2626(87)90126-6 [55] A. R. Lecours, et al., “Illiteracy and Brain Damage. 3: A Contribution to the Study of Speech and Language Dis- orders in Illiterates with Unilateral Brain Damage (Initial testing),” Neuropsychologia, Vol. 26, No. 4, 1988, pp. 575-589. do i:10.1016/0028-3932(88)90114-5 [56] F. Ostrosky, A. Ardila, M. Rosselli, G. Lopez-Arango and V. Uriel-Mendoza, “Neuropsychological Test Perfor- mance in Illiterates,” Archives of Clinical Neuropsychol- ogy, Vol. 13, No. 7, 1998, pp. 645-660. doi:10.1093/arclin/13.7.645
|