
N. JAHANGIRI ET AL. 629
Figure 9.
Jacquemot & Scott, 2006, p 481.
visual inputs therefore, the inputs can not access to phonologi-
cal store unless the participants can read and repeat them. 7%
within-list retrieval of patient group in overt rehearsal test sug-
gests that their vocal rehearsal process is weak. Also, it implys
that their articulatory subvocal rehearsal process is inactive too.
Use of the overt rehearsal procedure does not change the basic
effects observed in free recall (Tan & Ward, 2000, cited in
Brown et al, 2007, p257).
Therefore, when there is deficiency of articulatory rehearsal,
the possibility of memory traces vita will lose a few seconds
after the relevant word representation. Verbal language, that is,
auditory input, and writing language, that is, visual input, in-
teract with the working memory phonological loop. This stor-
age system includes two articulatory rehearsals and mainte-
nance of speech-based information. When an individual sees
and vocalizes pertinent word, and if the rehearsal part in the
memory phonological loop is active, this word will remain in
the maintenance part of this store. Repeating the word after
seeing and vocalizing, whether mentally or overt, is the reason
that word remains in the working memory phonological loop.
Every repetition itself is a lingual input; this auditory input
enters the working memory phonological loop and the inner
speech by means of continuous repetition, termed “maintenance
rehearsal,” protects that input in the memory. Then, the inner
speech role is to create a rehearsal state for maintaining the
memory phonological loop inputs. In this respect, rehearsal can
maintain only phonological representation, whereas refreshing
can maintain any representations through focus of attention.
Both of them aid in the maintenance of verbal information
(Camos et al., 2010). Participants were encouraged to apply the
cumulative rehearsal and there was no phonological similarity
within our list. Therefore all of the participants just operated
like the instruction. However, overt rehearsal should have the
necessary speed. Low speed of speech provides less rehearsal in
a limited time. Therefore, it provides fewer subsequent inputs
in patients than in healthy people. If we carry out this rehearsal
with a period of approximately 6 sec for gaps between the rep-
resentations in experimental conditions, we can find within-list
retrievals from the repetition ability and its subsequent, creating
latter inputs for the working memory phonological loop, which
they indicate as active rehearsal in the verbal memory. We
consider a human memory span limitation of 7 ± 2. Despite the
representation of 16 words which can be affected by the pref-
erence of the participants. The other researchers tested articula-
tory suppression effect on phonological loop in healthy people.
They tried to suppress rehearsal. Salame and Baddeley (1982)
claimed that articulatory suppression reduced overall levels of
serial recall because the items were unable to gain access to the
phonological store (Hanley, 1997, p424). Suppression during
visual input impedes rehearsal by occupying the rehearsal loop
(Toppino & Pisega, 2005, p375).
They suppressed rehearsal in healty people, but our findings
suggest that rehearsal decreses in dementia per se. The results
are the same, reduction of recall. On the other hand, the pri-
macy effect reflects the use of rehearsal to carry forward
early-list item selectively (Tan & Ward, 2000, cited in Brown et
al, 2007, p259). And the controls had such primacy effect until
the seventh word, but the patients had not. Through a compari-
son of the rehearsal manner of the two groups, impaired pri-
macy effect in group with dementia, it is concluded that the
pattern of overt rehearsal of the healthy group is different from
the pattern of patients suffering from dementia (Figures 2 and
6). Brown et al. (2007) reported classic amnesic pattern of sub-
stantially impaired primacy about one anterograde amnesiac
patient.
Conclusions
Subvocal rehearsal is the state of inner speech utilization,
which exists in normal older people, and we present it in ex-
perimental conditions. The passive rehearsal pattern is seen in
older people with dementia, who are not able to repeat stimulus
effectively and use inner speech. Thus, the rehearsal patterns of
the controls and the patients are different.
There are no primacy effects in the group with dementia, and
there are various repetition loses. In other words, the percentage
of the number of repetition belonging to previous words de-
clines, and the mean number of retrieved words per 15 posi-
tions included in the within-list retrievals significantly de-
creases. There is inner speech in the form of subvocal repetition
in normal old age individuals, and it is responsible for convert-
ing and maintenance of visual input to verbal silent input by
rehearsal. Therefore, the inner speech in the form of subvocal
repetition is a storage system, and also, it is possible that pri-
vate speech can be charged for the transition of visual input to
retrieval part of the phonological loop. Then, we deal with
whisper or more audible speech which can keep visual inputs in
memory phonological loop in the verbal repetition condition.
Then, private speech in the form of vocal repetition is a storage
system. Thus, deficiencies of inner speech and private speech
storage system will lesson the possibility of retrievals from the
phonological loop in older people affected by dementia.
References
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H.
Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 8.
London: Academic Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of
working memory? Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 417-423.
doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking
forward. Nature Reviews Neuros cience, 4, 829-839.
doi:10.1038/nrn1201
Baddeley, A. D., & Dlarsen, J. (2007). The phonological loop: Some
answers and some questions. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 60, 512-518. doi:10.1080/17470210601147663
Brown, D. A. G. (2007). Brief reports amnesia, rehearsal and temporal
distinctiveness models of recall. 256-260.
http://pbr.psychonomic-journals:content /14/2/256.abstract
Hanley, R. (1997). Does articulatory suppression remove the irrelevant
speech effect? Psyc h o l og y Press Memory, 5, 423-431.
Logie, R. H. (2003). Spatial and visual working memory―a mental
workspace. E lsevier Sci ence, 41-42.
Jacquemot, C., & Scott, K. S. (2006). What is the relation between
phonological short-term memory and speech processing?
http://www.sciencedirect.com, vol.10, 480-481.