Paper Menu >>
Journal Menu >>
Vol.3, No.8, 507-511 (2011) Health doi:10.4236/health.2011.38084 Copyright © 2011 SciRes. Openly accessible at http://www.scirp.org/journal/HEALTH/ Firmness of purpose in life significantly influences emotional state and autonomic nervous activity Riichiro Ishida*, Tsueko Abe, Masahiko Okada Division of Clinical Preventive Medicine, Niigata Graduate School of Medical and Dental Sciences, Niigata University, Niigata, Japan; *Corresponding Author: ishida-riichiro@hb.tp1.jp Received 23 April 2011; revised 2 June 2011; accepted 15 July 2011. ABSTRACT The present study was performed to clarify the influence of having a firm purpose in life (PIL) on psychological and physical stresses. Par- ticipants were 59 subjects (19 males and 40 fe- males) with a mean age of 30.9 years (standard deviation, 12.1; range, 18 - 68 years). We used a mental arithmetic (MA) test as a goal-oriented stressor; the test was supposed to be finished before a time limit expired without using pencil and paper. We used a PIL test to assess the in- tensity of personal meaning in one’s life, an in- terest in mental arithmetic test (IMAT) to assess the intensity of interest in the MA test, and an anxiety inventory (AI) to assess the a nxiety level of before and during the MA tes t. Heart rat e (HR), systolic blood pressure (SBP), diastolic blood pressure (DBP) and nose tip temperature (NTT) were recorded before and after MA. We used logistic regression modeling to estimate the odds ratio (OR) of independent variables of age, sex, PIL test score, AI score, HR, SBP, DBP, and NTT before the MA test. Multivariate-adjusted models showed that PIL test scores prior to the MA test significantly predicted the observed changes in AI score and HR. Other variables were not significant predictors. In conclusion, we found that a firm PIL decreased anxiety un- der psychological stress as measured with the MA test, even with objective evaluation by oth- ers. Having a firm PIL also lowered the response of the sympathetic nervous system to psycho- logical stress. Keywords: Purpose in Life; Psychological Stress; Anxiety; Sympathetic Nervous Activity; Longevity 1. INTRODUCTION Physical and psychological stresses can easily cause anxiety and simultaneously activate the sympathetic ner- vous system [1,2]. A “will” to seek meaning in life or to achieve purpose in life (PIL) is significantly associated with intrinsic motivation to get satisfaction and/or plea- sure from an act [3,4]. Hence, persons with a firm PIL are less sensitive to physical and psychological stresses [5]. People exhibiting a Type A behavior pattern, char- acterized by traits such as impatience, aggressiveness, strong approval motivation, and a chronic struggle to achieve one’s goal, are much more sensitive to stressors [6,7]. It has been reported that type A behavior tended to increase sympathetic nervous activity, generate reactive oxygen species, and cause secretion of -endorphin [1, 6-8]. It is also known that type A behavior is associated with increased risk of coronary heart disease (CHD) [7]. Several tests have been devised to measure the inten- sity of PIL. Crumbaugh and Maholic, for example, de- veloped a PIL test to assess the intensity of personal meaning in one’s life [9]; Sato and Tanaka modified this test for Japanese people [10]. This test enables meas- urement of intrinsic motivation, satisfaction, or pleasure in life. Audio-visual stimulations such as watching a kaleidoscopic roller coaster movie have been used to show that persons with firm PIL exhibited less anxiety and had lower sympathetic nervous system responses compared with persons with less firm PIL [5]. The stim- uli were physical and continuous without any appointed goal. In daily life, however, there are many goal-oriented stresses involving time limitations and being exposed to objective evaluation. For example, company employees have to accomplish a large number of tasks in a day, students have to complete their homework assignments, and housewives have to do many routine tasks every day. The present study was performed to clarify the influ- ence of having a firm PIL on responses to psychological and physical stresses. We hypothesized that people with a firm PIL show low anxiety and low sympathetic nerv- ous response to psychological stress. R. Ishida et al. / Health 3 (2011) 507-511 Copyright © 2011 SciRes. Openly accessible at http://www.scirp.org/journal/HEALTH/ 508 2. METHODS 2.1. Study Population Details of the subjects and the experimental protocol have been described previously [11]. Briefly, 62 subjects (20 males and 42 females) in a variety of occupations, including housewife, salesman, elementary school teacher, and technician, were recruited. Of these 62, 3 subjects whose attitude and physical symptoms could seriously affect the test results were excluded due to excessive talking during the test, frequent arrhythmia, or excessive time spent reading documents because of presbyopia. Finally, 59 subjects (19 males and 40 females) with a mean age of 30.9 years (standard deviation, 12.1; range, 18 - 68 years) participated in the study. The subjects were instructed not to drink alcohol or tea and not to smoke on the day of the experiment. No instructions were given regarding food intake. The test was performed on an individual basis. The ethics com- mittee of the Faculty of Medicine of Niigata University approved the study, and all subjects provided written informed consent. 2.2. Goal-Oriented Stress Test The goal-oriented stress we applied was a mental ari- thmetic (MA) test that was supposed to be finished within a given time limit without using pencil and paper. An instructor (male, 57 years of age) administered each test. First, the instructor explained the written protocol on the panel as follows: “Please perform the mental cal- culations as quickly and accurately as possible in four minutes. Answer the resulting values when I say ‘stop and answer.’ Do not talk during the test other than to answer with the resulting values during the test.” Then the instructor randomly chose one of three cards: “1000 – 6,” “1000 – 7,” or “1000 – 8,” and showed the card to the subject. Subjects were requested to perform sequen- tial subtractions as shown on the card. Thus, for example, a subject who received the “1000 – 6” card would cal- culate 1000 – 6 (= 994), 994 – 6 (= 988), 988 – 6 = 982, and so forth. 2.3. Psychological Tests The PIL test used here consists of 20 items to which subjects expressed agreement or disagreement on a 7-point Likert scale. The questionnaire is outlined in Appendix 1 [5,10]. High scores indicate a firm PIL [10]. The subjects were divided into groups according to their scores: firm PIL (FPIL) (55 points) and weak PIL (WPIL) (<55 points). We also devised another two tests. One was the inter- est in mental arithmetic test (IMAT) to assess the inten- sity of interest for the MA test. The IMAT consisted of only one question, “Are you interested in mental arith- metic?” and was scored on a 6-point Likert scale (1: not at all, 2: very weakly, 3: weakly, 4: somewhat strongly, 5: strongly, 6: very strongly). The other was an anxiety inventory (AI) to assess the anxiety level of before and during the MA test. The test before MA consisted of the question “Do you feel anxiety before MA?” and the test during MA was the question “Did you feel anxiety dur- ing MA?” These tests were each scored on a 6-point Likert scale (1: not at all, 2: very weakly, 3: weakly, 4: somewhat strongly, 5: strongly, 6: very strongly). 2.4. Measuring Autonomic Nervous Function After the subject had sat in a chair comfortably for 20 minutes, heart rate (HR), systolic blood pressure (SBP), and diastolic blood pressure (DBP) were recorded using an electric sphygmomanometer (ES-P1000, Terumo, Tokyo, Japan) and nose tip temperature (NTT) was measured with a skin thermometer (D717, Technol7m Tokyo, Japan). These measurements were performed before and after the MA test. As environmental condi- tions affect the autonomic nervous function, room tem- perature, humidity, illumination and noise were con- trolled at 24.51C 1.02C, 58.64% 4.61%, 259.17 78.21 lx and 53.10 1.37 dB, respectively. Atmospheric pressure was measured at the time of the experiment. 2.5. Statistical Analyses Univariate analyses were performed first to compare age, sex, PIL test score, IMAT score, and environmental conditions such as room temperature between FPIL and WPIL groups. Then AI score, HR, SBP, DBP and NTT were compared between values observed before and during/after MA tests. A chi-square test was used for evaluating differences in the number of men and women between the FPIL and WPIL groups. An unpaired 2-tailed t test was used to evaluate differences in age and environmental conditions such as room temperature between the FPIL and WPIL groups. The Mann-Whitney Test was used for evaluating differences in IMAT score between the FPIL and WPIL groups. The Wilcoxon signed rank test was used to eva- luate changes in the AI score from before to during the MA test. Paired 2-tailed t tests were used in evaluating changes of the four autonomic indicators HR, SBP, DBP, and NTT before and after the MA test. The basic char- acteristics were expressed as mean standard deviation (SD). Multivariate analyses were performed to evaluate changes before and during/after MA tests as follows. R. Ishida et al. / Health 3 (2011) 507-511 Copyright © 2011 SciRes. Openly accessible at http://www.scirp.org/journal/HEALTH/ 509 Change in AI score = (during MA test) – (before MA test) Change in HR, SBP and DBP = (after MA test) / (be- fore MA test) Change in NTT = (before MA test) / (after MA test). We used logistic regression modeling to estimate the odds ratios (OR) of independent variables for age, sex, PIL test score, AI score, HR, SBP, DBP, and NTT before the MA test. Subjects with PIL test score 55, AI score before MA 2.05, HR 64.03, SBP 107.49, DBP 72.88 or NTT ≤ 33.85 before MA were assigned a value of 1 for each respective independent variable; otherwise a value of 0 was assigned. For dependent variables, a value of 1 was assigned 1 for each subject whose score changes were: AI 1.10, HR 1.05, SBP 1.06, DBP 1.03, or NTT 1.02. Otherwise, a value of 0 was as- signed for that dependent variable. Statistical significance was accepted at p < 0.05, ex- cept for the tests of HR, SBP, DBP and NTT, which were accepted at p < 0.05 / 4 = 0.0125 by Bonferroni correc- tion. SPSS software was used for the statistical analyses. 3. RESULTS 3.1. Characteristics The basic characteristics of the subjects as sex, age, and IMAT score did not differ between the FPIL and WPIL groups (Ta b l e 1 ). Environmental conditions such as temperature and humidity were not significantly dif- ferent between FPIL and WPIL groups (data not shown). The recorded atmospheric pressures on the days of ex- periments were 1015.50 4.94 hPa, and did not differ between subject groups. 3.2. Anxiety and Autonomic Nervous Activity AI score, HR, SBP and DBP significantly increased during/after the MA test compared with the values be- fore the test, whereas NTT significantly decreased after the MA test (Table 2). Five logistic regression models for predicting the changes in AI score, HR, SBP, DBP and NTT are pre- sented in Table 3. The multivariate-adjusted models showed that the PIL score before the MA test was sig- nificant for predicting the change in AI score and the change in HR. The other variables of age, sex, AI score before MA, HR before MA test, SBP before MA test, DBP before MA test, and NTT before MA test were not significant in any of the multivariate-adjusted models. 4. DISCUSSION We found that MA was a possible psychological stressor because AI scores, HR, SBP, DBP and NTT all were changed significantly by the test (Ta b le 2). These results are compatible with previous reports that psy- chological stresses caused anxiety and also activated the sympathetic nervous system [12]. PIL score was nega- tively associated with change in AI score and change in HR (Table 3). Therefore, it can be concluded that the persons with a firm PIL showed lower anxiety and a lesser sympathetic nervous response to psychological Table 1. Demographic and psychological characteristics of subjects by categories of purpose in life (PIL). Characteristic FPIL group WPIL group p N (male/female) 26 (6/20) 33 (13/ 20) 0.183 Age (year) 30.19 ± 13.02 31.39 ± 11.50 0.708 PIL test score (point) 61.12 ± 4.32 47.24 ± 7.28 - IMAT score (point) 2.81 0.98 2.76 1.35 0.692 N: number of subjects, FPIL: firm PIL, WPIL: weak PIL, IMAT: interest in mental arithmetic test. Data are expressed as mean standard deviation. Ta b le 2. Anxiety inventory (AI) score, heart rate (HR), systolic blood pressure (SBP), diastolic blood pressure (DBP) and nose tip temperature (NTT) (N = 59). Changes Characteristic Before MA During/After MA p AI score (point) 2.05 1.32 3.20 1.54 <0.001 HR (beat/min) 64.03 9.86 67.59 12.14 <0.0025# SBP (mmHg) 107.49 15.87 112.86 16.73 <0.0025# DBP (mmHg) 72.88 10.29 74.98 10.47 <0.0125# NTT ()℃ 33.85 1.13 33.31 1.35 <0.0025# MA: mental arithmetic. #: statistically significant after Bonferroni correction for HR, SPB, DBP and NTT. R. Ishida et al. / Health 3 (2011) 507-511 Copyright © 2011 SciRes. Openly accessible at http://www.scirp.org/journal/HEALTH/ 510 Table 3. Adjusted odds ratios (OR) of demographic, psychological, and physiological characteristics (N = 59). Change in AI score Change in HR Change in SBPChange in DBP Change in NTT Characteristic OR p ORp ORP OR p ORp Age (year) 1.04 0.155 0.970.238 1.040.135 0.99 0.790 1.030.205 Sex 1.38 0.619 4.620.046 0.510.392 1.03 0.968 3.210.079 PIL test score before MA 0.27 <0.05 0.18<0.0125#0.760.662 0.96 0.939 0.330.076 AI score before MA 0.51 0.296 2.610.150 1.990.275 1.52 0.477 1.510.493 HR before MA 0.950.179 SBP before MA 0.970.229 DBP before MA 0.94 0.057 NTT before MA 0.930.780 #: statistically significant after Bonferroni correction. stress. The results are consistent with the previous study [5]. Much attention has been focused on the biological re- sponses caused by psychological stress [13-21]. Sympa- thetic nervous activity in response to stressors easily generates reactive oxygen species [1,2,22], which ace- lerate lipid oxidation, gene dysfunction, and telomere shortening [14-17]. It is interesting that the secretions of dopamine and -endorphin could be accelerated by the psychological reactions as reward, satisfaction, and/or pleasure based on intrinsic motivation or reinforcement of motivation [18-21]. A lower response against the psychological stresses with FPIL, therefore, could con- tribute to longevity. Limitations in this study should be noted. First, each of the AI tests before and during MA and the IMA test consisted of only one question. The tests should be vali- dated for the number of questions, sensitivity, specificity, and reproducibility in the future. Second, since the psy- chological condition of subjects might influence their test results, the experimental conditions, including in- structors’ attitudes, should be carefully discussed. Third, food intake before the experiment might influence the autonomic nervous system. Further studies with a large number of volunteers would be required. In conclusion, we found that a firm PIL decreased an- xiety during psychological stress caused by the MA test even under the objective evaluation by others. Having a firm PIL also lowered the response of the sympathetic nervous system to psychological stress. 5. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This study was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Science Research from the Japanese Society for the Promotion of Science (No.17927020; 2005). The authors wish to express their gratitude to Yuko Ishida for the data analysis. REFERENCES [1] Smith, E.E., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Fredrickson, B.K. and Loftus, G.R. (2003) Atkinson & Hilgard’s introduction to psychology. Thomson. [2] Atkinson, R.L., Atkinson, R.C., Smith, E.E., Ben, D.J. and Nolen-Hosksema, S. (1996) Hilgard’s introduction to psychology. Harcourt Brace College Publishers, Fort Worth. [3] Frankl, V.E. (1972) The meaning of meaninglessness: A challenge to psychotherapy. American Journal of Psy- choanalysis, 32, 85-89. doi:10.1007/BF01872487 [4] Bundra, A. (1977) Social learning theory. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River. [5] Ishida, R. and Okada, M. (2006) Effects of a firm pur- pose in life on anxiety and sympathetic nervous activity caused by emotional stress: Assessment by psycho-phy- siological method. Stress and Health, 22, 275-281. doi:10.1002/smi.1095 [6] Agbu, J.F. (2010) Type A behavior pattern: A new insight to gender challenges in higher education. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 15, 528-539. doi:10.1080/13548506.2010.487106 [7] Hemingway, H., Shipley, M., Brunner, E., Britton, A., Malik, A. and Marmot, M. (2005) Does autonomic func- tion link social position to coronary risk? Circulation, 111, 3071-3077. doi:10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.104.497347 [8] Atanackovic, D., Schulze, J., kröger, H., Brunner-Wei- nzierl, M.C. and Deter, H.C. (2003) Acute psychological stress induces a prolonged suppression of the production of reactive oxygen species by phagocytes. Journal of Neuroimmunology, 142, 159-165. doi:10.1016/S0165-5728(03)00267-4 [9] Crumbaugh, J.C. and Maholic, L.T. (1964) An experi- mental study in existentialism: The psychometric ap- proach to Frankl’s concept of noogenic neurosis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 20, 200-207. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(196404)20:2<200::AID-JCLP22 70200203>3.0.CO;2-U [10] Sato, F. and Tanaka, H. (1974) An experimental study on the existential aspect of life: Part I. Tohoku Psychologica Folia. 33, 20-46. [11] Ishida, R. (2008a) Correlation between social desirability and autonomic nervous function under goal-oriented stress (mental arithmetic) with consideration of parental attitude (in Japanese). Autonomic Nervous System, 45, 242-249. [12] Smith, E.E., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Fredrickson, B.L. and Loftus, G.R. (2003) Atkinson & Hilgard’s introduction to R. Ishida et al. / Health 3 (2011) 507-511 Copyright © 2011 SciRes. Openly accessible at http://www.scirp.org/journal/HEALTH/ 511 psychology. Thomson. [13] Tannno, K., Sakata, K., Ohsawa, M., Onoda, T., Itai, K., Yaegashi, Y. and Tamakoshi, A. (2009) Association of ikigai as a positive psychological factor with all-cause mortality and cause-specific mortality among middle- aged and elderly Japanese people: Finding from the Ja- pan collaborative cohort study. Journal of Psychoso- matic Research, 67, 67-75. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.10.018 [14] Willls, L.P. and Schnellmann, R.G. (2011) Telomeres and telomerase in renal health. Journal of the American Soci- ety of Nephrology, 22, 39-41. doi:10.1681/ASN.2010060662 [15] Ben-Porath, I. and Weinberger, R.A. (2005) The signals and pathways activating cellular senescence. The Inter- national Journal of Biochemistry & Cell Biology, 37, 961-976. doi:10.1016/j.biocel.2004.10.013 [16] Bartholomew, J.N. and Galbiati, F. (2010) Mapping of oxidative stress response elements of the caveolin-1 pro- moter. Methods in molecular biology, 594, 409-423. doi:10.1007/978-1-60761-411-1_29 [17] Ishii, N., Ishii, T. and Hartman, P.S. (2007) The role of the electron transport SDHC gene on lifespan and cancer. Mitochondrion, 7, 24-28. doi:10.1016/j.mito.2006.11.012 [18] Stefano, G.B., Bianchi, E., Guarma, M., Fricchione, G.L., Zhu, W., Cadet, P., Mantione, K.J., Casares, F.M., Kream, R.M. and Esch, T. (2007) Nicotine, alcohol and cocaine coupling to reward processes via endogenous morphine signaling: The dopamine-morphine. Medical Science Mo- nitor: International Medical Journal of Experimental and Clinical Research, 13, 91-102. [19] Xu, X., Aron, A., Brown, L., Cao, G., Feng, T. and Weng, X. (2011) Reward and motivation system: A brain map- ping study of early-stage intense romantic love in Chi- nese participants. Human Brain Mapping, 32, 249-257. doi:10.1002/hbm.21017 [20] Hegadoren, K.M., O’Donnell, T., Lanius, R., Coupland, N.J. and Lacaze-Masmonteil, N. (2009) The role of beta- endorphin in the pathophysiology of major depression. Neuropeptides, 43, 341-353. doi:10.1016/j.npep.2009.06.004 [21] Spanagel, R., Herz, A., Bals-Kubik, R. and Shippenberg, T.S. (1991) Beta-endorphin-induced locomotor stimula- tion and reinforcement are associated with an increase in dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens. Psy- chopharmacology, 104, 51-56. doi:10.1007/BF02244553 [22] Touyz, R.M. and Briones, A.M. (2011) Reactive oxygen species and vascular biology: Implications in human hy- pertension. Hypertension Research, 34, 5-14. doi:10.1038/hr.2010.201 Appendix 1. Examples of the questions used in the purpose in life (PIL) test. (1) In life I have: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 (no goals or aims at all) (neutral) (very clear goals and aims) (2) Concerning man’s freedom to make his own choices, I believe man is: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 (completely bound by limitations (neutral) (absolutely free to make all choices) of heredity and environment) (3) Every day is 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 (exactly the same) (neutral) (constantly new and different) (4) My life is: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 (empty, filled only with despair) (neutral) (running over with exciting and good things) (5) If I should die today, I would feel that my life has been: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 (completely worthless) (neutral) (very worthwhile) |