 Psychology 2011. Vol.2, No.2, 71-77 Copyright © 2011 SciRes. DOI:10.4236/psych.2011.22012 Romantic Relationship Status and Gender Differences in Sun Tanning Attitudes and Behaviors of U.S. College Students Terry F. Pettijohn II1, Terry F. Pettijohn2, Alexandra G. Gilbert1 1Department of Psychology, Coastal Carolina University, Conway, South Carolina, USA; 2Department of Psychology, the Ohio State University-Marion, Marion, Ohio, USA. Email: pettijohn@coastal.edu Received November 13th, 2010; revised January 25th, 2011; accepted February 10th, 2011. Male and female college students (n = 353) involved in varying degrees of romantic relationships responded to a questionnaire investigating sun tanning attitudes and behaviors. Females were predicted to engage in sun tanning more frequently and have more positive attitudes about sun tanning than males. Those students who were dating were also predicted to engage in more sun tanning behaviors and have more positive attitudes regarding tanning compared to students who were not dating, in committed romantic relationships, or married. Results supported these predictions overall. Females, and college students who were dating, engaged in more risky sun tanning behaviors and are therefore especially at risk for health related consequences of sun tanning. Keywords: Sun Tanning, Gender Differences, Romantic Relationships, Skin Cancer Introduction Open any contemporary fashion magazine in the United States and you will find photographs and advertisements dis- playing “sun-kissed beauties” or “bronzed goddesses” portray- ing standards of beauty. When and why did this trend of having a sun tan become so popular? Pale skin used to mark a status symbol of wealth and beauty (Mighall, 2008). Literature re- ferred to women as “fair maidens” and fashioned images like the iconic Disney Princesses, who possess creamy, white skin. Tan colored skin represented the lower class of manual laborers who worked outside in the sun (Righton, 2005). Sun exposure was first introduced in the early 20th century by the medical and scientific communities for the treatment of tuberculosis and rickets; in 1903 Niels Finsen was awarded the Nobel Peace Prize in Medicine for treatment with heliotherapy. During this time, large hats, parasols, and bleaching cream were used by the upper class to avoid sun tanning and the stigma attached to tan skin (Mighall, 2008). In the Great Depression, with scarce food and low paying manual labor jobs, full figured bodies and pale skin represented the health of having plenty to eat, and the upper class wealth of not working in manual labor occupations outdoors. Magazines like Vogue and Harpers Bazaar, and fashion icon Coco Chanel, created tan as an ideal of attractiveness in the late 1920s (Mighall, 2008). In the months of May, June, and July from the years of 1920 to 1927 in Vogue and Harpers Bazaar, there are only three pro-tan articles, and two pro-tan advertise- ments (Martin, et al., 2009). English Vogue in 1927 had the first tan model on the cover (Mighall, 2008). During the same months, in 1928 to 1930, there were 30 pro-tan articles and 99 advertisements (Martin et al., 2009), showing how the media influenced tan, and a sun tan became revolutionized as healthy and attractive. Banerjee, Campo, and Greene (2008) researched the per- ceived attractiveness of males and females with a tan. Re- searchers used photographs of the same models and altered their appearance to have light, medium, and dark sun tans. Males rated the female model with the dark tan as healthier and more attractive, while females did not show a significant pref- erence for males with tans. Females reported that being attrac- tive to men was a strong factor in their lifestyles, influencing diet choices, working out, and tanning. Females believed that sun tanning increases their physical and interpersonal attrac- tiveness to males and sun tanning also makes them look health- ier. Females often engage in the forewarned beauty risks of tanning, injections, and plastic surgery. The risks of altering aesthetic attributes are outweighed by the desire to increase their perceived attractiveness to find a mate. Males find tan females more attractive, approachable (romantically or friendly), and healthy (Benerjee et al., 2008). Other researchers have also found an attractiveness preference for tan skin (Broadstock, Borland, & Gason, 1992; Smith, Cornelissen, & Tovée, 2007). The association of sun tan equaling healthy is where the paradox of sun tanning comes in. Skin cancer is now the most commonly occurring cancer in the United States (Lamanna, 2004), with more than two million people receiving treatment for basal and squamous cell skin cancer in 2006 (American Cancer Society, 2010). Researchers have also shown that de- spite the increase in education of skin cancer, the rate of college student sun tanning has increased in recent years (Pettijohn, Pettijohn, & Geschke, 2009). How can the dangerous sun tan- ning craze be explained, since it runs counter to ideals of health and self-preservation? Past studies have shown how certain ideals of attractiveness, such as skin tone and body shape, have changed across time (Mighall, 2008). In addition, evolutionary theory (Buss, 1994; Saad & Peng, 2006) can be used to explore why there is a preference for tan skin in mate selection. Evolutionary psychology proposes that beauty preferences have evolved due to the indication of health and fertility factors for men, and the reproductive drive of finding a mate for women (Buss, 1994; Saad & Peng, 2006). Skin tone is an im-
 T. F. PETTIJOHN II ET AL. 72 portant signal of attractiveness because it is a visible indicator of health and fertility. Men will hunt to find youth and beauty in their mate so their offspring potentially carry these inherit- able traits (Buss, 1994; Saad & Peng, 2006). Demographically, young single women from the ages of 16 to 29 years old make up the largest faction of sun tanning patrons and dating females (Lamanna, 2004). While seeking a mate, women are more fo- cused on the present factors that enhance their reproductive potential. On a college sun tanning inventory, 82.1% placed a high value on sun tans, and 72.8% indicated they understood the seriousness of skin cancer (Lamanna, 2004). Females had higher risk taking behaviors in sun tanning, rated the perceived risk of tanning higher, and were more knowledgeable about skin cancer than males (Lamanna, 2004). The risk behavior in sun tanning follows the life history theory that women are more likely to engage in risks if it will elevate their attractiveness level (Saad & Peng, 2006). With females engaging in the risk taking behavior of sun tan- ning to increase their chances to find a mate, they outweigh the perceived risks of skin cancer in the future for the immediate benefits of perceived health and attractiveness (Cokkinides et al., 2002; Leary & Jones, 1993). The specific demographic group that is affected by this is the young, single dating, female population (Saad & Peng, 2006). Females who focus on the immediate attractiveness enhancing effects of tanning disregard the predictive costs. These behavioral intentions fall in line with the Darwinian perspective of mate selection. Current Study Predictions Based on these previous findings, the researchers predicted females would report greater frequency of sun tanning behav- iors and more positive attitudes regarding sun tanning com- pared to males. In addition, single students who were dating were expected to show more positive attitudes towards sun tanning and report a greater frequency of sun tanning behaviors compared to students who were in committed relationships or not dating. Married individuals were expected to show the least positive attitudes about sun tanning and report the least fre- quency of sun tanning behaviors. Students who are dating should place a high premium on appearance and therefore be most concerned with maintaining a perceived healthy sun tan to attract and keep a mate. Married students have already attracted a mate and established a relationship, so these individuals should be least concerned with maintaining a sun tan. Research has found that being involved in romantic relationships may protect individuals from body esteem threats and increase per- sonal acceptance and self-beliefs (Forbes, Jobe, & Richardson, 2006; Lin & Kulik, 2002; Long, 1983) which may reduce the need to focus on self-enhancement behaviors, such as tanning. Method Participants Three hundred fifty three (62.8% female) undergraduate col- lege students from a large, public university in the state of Ohio participated in this research. The sample was 95.2% Caucasian, 1.1% Asian, 1.1% African-American, 2% Hispanic, and .6% other. The average age of the sample was 21.50 years (SD = 6.89, range = 18 – 68) and 65.6% were freshmen, 13.9% sophomores, 8.2% juniors, 8.2% seniors, and 4.0% other. The reported romantic relationship status of the students included 24.6% single non-dating, 34.6% single dating, 29.7% single committed, and 11.0% married. Materials & Procedure Students enrolled in psychology courses volunteered to com- plete the sun tanning survey. The survey consisted of 24 sun tanning attitude questions ( = .77; SA1-24) which were an- swered using a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neutral, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly disagree) and 12 sun tanning frequency questions ( = .84; For SF1-8, 1 = never, 2 = once a month, 3 = once a week, 4 = two times a week, 5 = three or more times a week. For SF9-12, 1 = never, 2 = almost never, 3 = sometimes, 4 = almost always, 5 = always). These sun tanning questions were used in a previous investigation by Pettijohn, Pettijohn, and Geschke (2009). Participants were also asked “Do you regularly engage in sun tanning behavior?” and “Do you believe individuals prefer romantic partners with sun tans?” as yes/no questions. Demographic questions were in- cluded to provide information about participant age, sex, race, year in college, and romantic relationship status. Results A 2 (Sex: male or female) × 4 (Relationship status: single non-dating, single dating, single committed, or married) MANOVA for the sun tanning attitude questions (SA1-24) was conducted. Results revealed a significant main effect for sex, Wilks’ = 0.75, F(24 312) = 4.32, p < .001, 2 = .25, a signifi- cant main effect for relationship status, Wilks’ = 0.75, F(72 933) = 1.30, p = .05, 2 = .09, and a non-significant interaction effect, Wilks’ = 0.82, F(72 933) = 0.89, p = .71, 2 = .07. Individual comparisons, using a Bonferroni correction, were conducted for participant sex (see Table 1). Males scored sig- nificantly higher than females on SA2, SA3, SA4, SA9, SA10, SA12, SA13, SA16, SA17, and SA19. Females scored signifi- cantly higher than males on SA5, SA20, and SA21. One way ANOVAs were conducted to investigate the differ- ences among relationship status for each SA question (see Ta- ble 2). Significant differences between relationship statuses were found for SA5, SA6, SA16, and SA23, while marginally significant differences were found for SA1, SA3, and SA22. Individual comparisons, using a Bonferroni correction, were also conducted for relationship status and the results are pre- sented in Table 2. A 2 (Sex: male or female) × 4 (Relationship status: single non-dating, single dating, single committed, or married) MANOVA for the sun tanning behavior frequency questions (SF1-12) was conducted. Results revealed a significant main effect for sex, Wilks’ = 0.85, F(12 318) = 4.76, p < .001, 2 = .15, a significant main effect for relationship status, Wilks’ = 0.83, F(36 940) = 1.76, p < .01, 2 = .06, and a non-significant interaction effect, Wilks’ = 0.91, F(36 940) = 0.84, p = .74, 2 = .03. Individual comparisons, using a Bonferroni correction, were conducted for participant sex (see Table 3). Females scored significantly higher than males on SF1, SF3, SF5, SF7, SF10, SF11, and SF12. Males did not score significantly higher than females on any of the questions. One way ANOVAs investigating the differences among rela-
 T. F. PETTIJOHN II ET AL. 73 tionship status were also conducted for each SF question (see Table 4). Significant differences between relationship statuses were found for SF1, SF3, SF5, SF6, SF7, SF10, and SF11. In- dividual comparisons, using a Bonferroni correction, were also conducted for relationship status. Results are presented in Table 4. To analyze the question “Do you regularly engage in sun tanning behavior?”, two chi-square tests for independence were conducted to investigate how participant sex and relationship status were related to question responses. There was a signifi- cant relationship between participant sex and question re- sponses, 2(1, N = 352) = 18.49, p < .001, φ = 0.23. Females reported regularly engaging in sun tanning behavior (69.7%) more than males (46.6%). There was also a significant rela- tionship between relationship status and question responses, 2(3, N = 353) = 12.29, p < .01, φ = 0.19. As can be seen in Figure 1, single dating participants sun tan more regularly than other relationship status types. To analyze the question “Do you believe individuals prefer romantic partners with sun tans?” two chi-square tests for in- dependence were conducted to investigate how participant sex and relationship status were related to question responses. There was a significant relationship between participant sex and question responses, 2(1, N = 352) = 17.39, p < .001, φ = 0.22. Males believed individuals prefer romantic partners with sun tans (66.4%) more than females did (43.3%). There was also a significant relationship between relationship status and question response, 2(3, N = 353) = 19.08, p < .001, φ = 0.23. As can be seen in Figure 2, single dating participants believed individuals prefer romantic partners with a sun tan more than the other relationship status types. To consider the influence of age on sun tanning attitudes and behaviors, we correlated age with SA1-24 and SF1-12. There was a significant positive correlation between age and SA20, SA21, SA22, SA23, ps < 0.05. There was a significant negative correlation between age and SF1, SF10, and SF11, ps < 0.05. Discussion Overall, the hypotheses were generally supported. With re- spect to sun tanning behavior, females reported significantly greater frequency of regularly sun tanning and sun tanning Table 1. Mean college student sun tanning attitude responses by sex. Question Male Female SA1. I look more attractive with a sun tan than without a sun tan. 3.81 (0.86) 3.89 (1.02) SA2. Sun tanned skin is more attractive than skin that is not sun tanned.* 3.70 (0.89) 3.50 (0.94) SA3. Men engage in sun tanning behavior more frequently than women.* 3.50 (0.83) 3.29 (0.94) SA4. Suntans look healthy.* 3.66 (0.94) 3.22 (0.989) SA5. I enjoy engaging in sun tanning behavior.* 2.91 (1.11) 3.46 (1.18) SA6. It is important for me to have a sun tan. 2.68 (1.02) 2.79 (1.06) SA7. Suntanned men are more attractive than men without sun tans. 3.06 (0.82) 3.21 (1.02) SA8. I look more attractive with a sun tan than without a sun tan. 3.69 (0.92) 3.78 (1.05) SA9. Sun tanned individuals go out on more dates than individuals without sun tans.* 2.69 (0.84) 2.32 (0.82) SA10. Sun tanned individuals have more fun than individuals without sun tans.* 2.32 (0.88) 2.06 (0.82) SA11. When I have a sun tan, I feel more attractive than when I do not have a sun tan. 3.44 (0.98) 3.65 (1.05) SA12. Men prefer to date women who have sun tans over women who do not have sun tans.* 2.40 (0.82) 2.05 (0.77) SA13. I look more attractive without a sun tan than with a sun tan.* 4.24 (0.78) 3.92 (0.82) SA14. Sun tanned individuals are healthier than individuals without sun tans. 3.36 (0.89) 3.18 (1.02) SA15. Women engage in sun tanning behavior more frequently than men. 2.54 (0.91) 2.38 (0.93) SA16. Sun tanned women are more attractive than women without sun tans.* 2.45 (0.94) 2.06 (0.79) SA17. Individuals who pursue sun tans are too concerned with outward appearances.* 3.13 (0.96) 2.76 (0.79) SA18. It is important for my boyfriend/girlfriend to have a sun tan. 1.87 (0.67) 2.03 (0.59) SA19. Women prefer to date men who have sun tans over men who do not have sun tans.* 3.06 (0.81) 2.54 (0.84) SA20. I am concerned with premature aging associated with sun tanning behavior.* 3.26 (0.93) 3.64 (0.94) SA21. I am concerned about getting skin cancer from exposure to UV rays.* 3.40 (1.03) 3.76 (0.91) SA22. Sunscreens should be used when an individual engages in sun tanning behavior. 3.96 (0.77) 4.03 (0.81) SA23. Sun tanning enhancers (i.e. baby oil, deep tanning lotions) should never be used. 2.88 (0.88) 2.81 (1.04) SA24. Being tan now is more important to me than the risk of skin cancer in the future. 2.13 (0.85) 2.03 (0.85) Note. * p < .05. Standard deviations are presented in parentheses (SD). For SA1-24, 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neutral, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly disagree.
 T. F. PETTIJOHN II ET AL. 74 Table 2. Mean college student sun tanning attitude responses by romantic relationship sta tu s. Question Single Non-datingSingle Dating Single CommittedMarried SA1. I look more attractive with a sun tan than without a sun tan.a 3.67 (0.97) 4.02 (0.86) 3.87 (0.99) 3.81 (1.10) SA2. Sun tanned skin is more attractive than skin that is not sun tanned. 3.45 (0.95) 3.68 (0.93) 3.52 (0.86) 3.66 (0.99) SA3. Men engage in sun tanning behavior more frequently than women. 3.31 (0.87) 3.44 (0.97) 3.24 (0.84) 3.58 (0.89) SA4. Suntans look healthy.a 3.17 (0.99)1 3.55 (0.98)2 3.34 (0.95) 3.47 (1.0) SA5. I enjoy engaging in sun tanning behavior.* 2.91 (1.21)1 3.42 (1.12)2 3.40 (1.14)2 3.16 (1.29) SA6. It is important for me to have a sun tan.* 2.59 (1.08) 2.95 (1.02) 2.67 (0.98) 2.71 (1.18) SA7. Suntanned men are more attractive than men without sun tans. 3.07 (0.92) 3.13 (0.93) 3.18 (1.02) 3.37 (0.94) SA8. I look more attractive with a sun tan than without a sun tan. 3.59 (1.01) 3.85 (0.95) 3.75 (0.98) 3.79 (1.17) SA9. Sun tanned individuals go out on more dates than individuals without sun tans. 2.51 (0.89) 2.50 (0.86) 2.38 (0.82) 2.42 (0.79) SA10. Sun tanned individuals have more fun than individuals without sun tans. 2.22 (0.87) 2.11 (0.89) 2.10 (0.85) 2.29 (0.77) SA11. When I have a sun tan, I feel more attractive than when I do not have a sun tan. 3.40 (0.99) 3.64 (1.02) 3.67 (1.00) 3.53 (1.16) SA12. Men prefer to date women who have sun tans over women who do not have sun tans.2.15 (0.73) 2.24 (0.78) 2.22 (0.89) 1.97 (0.82) SA13. I look more attractive without a sun tan than with a sun tan. 4.10 (0.84) 3.98 (0.82) 4.08 (0.79) 3.95 (0.87) SA14. Sun tanned individuals are healthier than individuals without sun tans. 3.38 (1.00) 3.13 (0.94) 3.25 (1.00) 3.29 (0.96) SA15. Women engage in sun tanning behavior more frequently than men. 2.59 (0.94)1 2.39 (0.95) 2.49 (0.89) 2.08 (0.85)2 SA16. Sun tanned women are more attractive than women without sun tans.* 2.23 (.89) 2.38 (.89)1 1.99 (.83)2 2.16 (0.75) SA17. Individuals who pursue sun tans are too concerned with outward appearances. 2.87 (0.93) 2.92 (0.91) 2.92 (0.82) 2.79 (0.78) SA18. It is important for my boyfriend/girlfriend to have a sun tan. 2.01 (0.66) 1.99 (0.62) 1.92 (0.61) 1.95 (0.57) SA19. Women prefer to date men who have sun tans over men who do not have sun tans.2.83 (0.88) 2.78 (0.80) 2.68 (0.91) 2.53 (0.86) SA20. I am concerned with premature aging associated with sun tanning behavior. 3.50 (0.94) 3.47 (0.95) 3.39 (0.95)1 3.87 (0.91)2 SA21. I am concerned about getting skin cancer from exposure to UV rays. 3.64 (0.93) 3.60 (0.99) 3.58 (1.02) 3.82 (0.87) SA22. Sunscreens should be used when an individual engages in sun tanning behavior.a 4.15 (0.81) 3.88 (0.84)1 3.91 (0.73)1 4.32 (0.66)2 SA23. Sun tanning enhancers (i.e. baby oil, deep tanning lotions) should never be used.*2.99 (0.98) 2.68 (0.94)1 2.77 (0.97) 3.13 (1.10)2 SA24. Being tan now is more important to me than the risk of skin cancer in the future. 2.06 (0.80) 2.16 (0.89) 2.00 (0.80) 1.97 (0.94) Note. ap < .10, * p < .05. Standard deviations are presented in parentheses (SD). Significant individual comparison results are denoted by non-matching numbers within category rows, p < .05. For SA1-24, 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neutral, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly disagree. Table 3. Mean college student sun tanning behavior responses by sex. Question Male Female SF1. In the winter, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior indoors (e.g. tanning bed or lamp)?* 1.37 (0.93) 1.90 (1.28) SF2. In the winter, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior outdoors? 1.21 (0.77) 1.15 (0.58) SF3. In the spring, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior indoors (e.g. tanning bed or lamp)?* 1.39 (1.00) 2.29 (1.50) SF4. In the spring, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior outdoors? 2.10 (1.33) 2.03 (1.22) SF5. In the summer, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior indoors (e.g. tanning bed or lamp)?* 1.28 (0.80) 1.64 (1.16) SF6. In the summer, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior outdoors? 3.01 (1.61) 3.52 (1.36) SF7. In the autumn, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior indoors (e.g. tanning bed or lamp)?* 1.31 (.85) 1.81 (1.22) SF8. In the autumn, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior outdoors? 1.89 (1.29) 1.63 (1.07) SF9. How often do you use sunscreen when engaging in sun tanning behavior? 1.83 (1.18) 2.05 (1.33) SF10. How often do you use sun tanning enhancers (i.e. baby oil, deep tanning lotions) when engaging in sun tanning behavior?* 1.71 (1.17) 2.43 (1.42) SF11. How often do you engage in sun tanning behavior for special events (e.g. weddings, dances, social events)?* 1.96 (1.24) 2.98 (1.32) SF12. How often do you use artificial sun tan products (i.e. lotions, sprays, or pills)?* 1.25 (0.65) 1.78 (1.06) Note. * p < .05. Standard deviations are presented in parentheses (SD). For SF1-8, 1 = never, 2 = once a month, 3 = once a week, 4 = two times a week, 5 = three or more times a week. For SF9-12, 1 = never, 2 = almost never, 3 = sometimes, 4 = alm ost alw ays, 5 = always.
 T. F. PETTIJOHN II ET AL. 75 Table 4. Mean college student sun tanning behavior respo nses by romantic relationship st at u s. Question Single Non-datingSingle Dating Single CommittedMarried SF1. In the winter, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior indoors (e.g. tanning bed or lamp)?* 1.30 (0.66)1 1.95 (1.32)2 1.91 (1.35)2 1.30 (0.85)1 SF2. In the winter, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior outdoors?* 1.05 (0.27)1 1.20 (0.66) 1.16 (0.66) 1.43 (1.07)2 SF3. In the spring, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior indoors (e.g. tanning bed or lamp)?* 1.40 (0.98)1 2.26 (1.50)2 2.16 (1.49)2 1.76 (1.38) SF4. In the spring, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior outdoors? 1.92 (1.25) 1.99 (1.23) 2.23 (1.27) 2.11 (1.31) SF5. In the summer, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior indoors (e.g. tanning bed or lamp)?* 1.23 (0.70)1 1.70 (1.23)2 1.57 (1.12) 1.38 (0.86) SF6. In the summer, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior outdoors?* 2.82 (1.54)1 3.42 (1.49)2 3.61 (1.33)2 3.43 (1.39) SF7. In the autumn, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior indoors (e.g. tanning bed or lamp)?* 1.33 (0.78)1 1.78 (1.22)2 1.80 (1.26)2 1.35 (0.89) SF8. In the autumn, how often do you engage in sun tanning behavior outdoors? 1.59 (1.08) 1.80 (1.22) 1.77 (1.20) 1.65 (1.03) SF9. How often do you use sunscreen when engaging in sun tanning behavior? 1.93 (1.31) 1.85 (1.14) 2.10 (1.38) 2.08 (1.34) SF10. How often do you use sun tanning enhancers (i.e. baby oil, deep tanning lotions) when engaging in sun tanning behavior?* 1.83 (1.21)1 2.47 (1.44)2 2.31 (1.44)2 1.54 (.96)1 SF11. How often do you engage in sun tanning behavior for special events (e.g. weddings, dances, social events)?* 2.08 (1.29)1 2.79 (1.37)2 2.93 (1.38)2 2.32 (1.25) SF12. How often do you use artificial sun tan products (i.e. lotions, sprays, or pills)? 1.55 (0.89) 1.57 (0.95) 1.57 (1.00) 1.76 (1.09) Note. * p < .05. Standard deviations are presented in parentheses (SD). Significant individual comparison results are denoted by non-matching numbers within category rows, p < .05. For SF1-8, 1 = never, 2 = o nce a month, 3 = once a week, 4 = two times a week, 5 = three or more times a week. For SF9-12, 1 = never, 2 = almost never, 3 = sometimes, 4 = almost always, 5 = always. Figure 1. Figure 2. Percentage of respondents who regularly sun tan by relationship stat us. Percentage of respondents who believe individuals prefer romantic partners with sun tans by relationship status. indoors during all seasons compared to males. Females also reported a higher frequency of using sun tanning enhancers, tanning for special events, and using artificial tanning products than their male counterparts. We were surprised to see similar reported sun tanning behaviors between men and women out- doors. While few individuals reported sun tanning outdoors in the winter, fall, and spring, females reported tanning more often than males in the summer, but this difference was not statisti- cally significant. There were also no significant differences in sunscreen use between men and women in our sample. Older individuals reported tanning less often indoors in the winter, using sun tanning enhancers, and tanning for special events. trend consistent with predictions for single dating individuals to sun tan most frequently and married or single non-dating indi- viduals to sun tan least frequently. Single dating individuals also reported using sun tan enhancers most often, while married individuals reported using sun tan enhancers least often. Single non-dating individuals were more similar to married individuals in their sun tanning behavior, which may be related to their non-participation in dating. If dating is not important, individu- als do not need to engage in behaviors (i.e., sun tanning) to enhance their appearance to attract a potential mate. Individuals in dating relationships may also be more concerned and un- happy with body satisfaction compared to individuals in com- mitted relationships or individuals not dating (Forbes, Jobe, & While considering relationship status, the pattern of results for sun tanning behaviors were more varied, despite a general
 T. F. PETTIJOHN II ET AL. 76 Richardson, 2006; Lin & Kulik, 2002; Long, 1983). Brandberg, Ullen, Sjoberg, and Holm (1998) found that females with lower body satisfaction sun tanned more than females with higher body satisfaction and Leary and Jones (1993) found that those interested in making a favorable appearance impression were most likely to sun tan. The sun tanning attitudes results were somewhat mixed over- all. While females reported more positive attitudes regarding the experience of sun tanning compared to males, they were also more concerned with skin cancer and premature aging. Males reported more positive attitudes related to the enhanced appearance of sun tanned individuals over individuals without a sun tan. Men’s beliefs that sun tans are healthy, attractive, and desirable in mates is consistent with previous research demon- strating these preferences (e.g., Benerjee et al., 2008). While males may not go out of their way to engage in purposeful sun tanning behavior, or enjoy the sun tanning experience, they do recognize the importance of a sun tan in mate attraction. With the variable of relationship status, the pattern of results for sun tanning attitudes is also varied, despite a general trend consistent with predictions for single dating individuals to hold more positive sun tanning attitudes and married or single non-dating individuals to hold less positive sun tanning atti- tudes. For example, single dating individuals reported the greatest preferences for romantic partners with sun tans, thought they themselves look more attractive with a sun tan, thought tans look healthy, and believed it is important to have a sun tan, whereas married and single non-dating individuals reported the least agreement with these statements. Single dat- ing individuals were the least likely to agree that sun tanning enhancers should never be used and sunscreens should be used when sun tanning, while single non-dating and married indi- viduals were more likely to support these attitude statements. Older individuals reported more concern about premature aging and getting skin cancer from tanning as well as a negative atti- tude about the use of sun tanning enhancers. We would like to draw attention to some of the limitations of the current investigation. We acknowledge that we only as- sessed reported behaviors and attitudes of college students and that actual behavior may not directly correspond with these responses (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977). Special circumstances, social desirability, peer pressure, and social norms may influ- ence sun tanning behaviors in real world settings (Stapleton, Turrisi, & Hillhouse, 2008). Additional observations of sun tanning frequencies, sunscreen purchases, and other tanning behaviors would be a welcome addition to the current results. It should also be noted that partners in a relationship may provide a strong motivation for reducing risky sun tanning activities, such as sunscreen use (Mosher & Danoff-Burg, 2008). Furthermore, our sample consisted of college students from the Midwest section of the United States. While college is an ideal setting to explore romantic relationships and their influ- ences on other variables due to the large number of similar individuals in close proximity, we cannot extend our results to non-college populations or areas outside the Midwest. It would be interesting to assess sun tanning attitudes and behaviors from a wide range of ages and geographic locations to provide further clarification of results. Alternative measures with dem- onstrated reliability and validity may also be used to accurately measure specific dimensions of sun tanning attitudes and planned behaviors (e.g., Cafri et al., 2008). In addition, our sample was almost exclusively heterosexual, but the sun tan- ning attitudes and behavior of varying sexual orientations would be interesting to explore in the future (Reilly & Rudd, 2008). Understanding sex differences and the effect of relationship status on sun tanning attitudes and behaviors is important in determining individuals most at risk for skin cancer. Specifi- cally, the current research indicates females, and individuals who are dating, are especially at risk for health related conse- quences of sun tanning. Since sun tans are so valued among these groups, future research regarding methods to reduce these unhealthy attitudes and preferences are recommended. Acknowledgements The authors thank Kimberly LaPiene, Lauren Dickey, and Arsida Ndoni for helpful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript. References Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theo- retical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Bul- letin, 84, 888-918. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.84.5.888 American Cancer Society (2010). Cancer facts and figures. Atlanta, GA: American Cancer Society. Banerjee, S., Campo, S., & Greene, K. (2008). Fact or wishful thinking? Biased expectations in ‘I think I look better when I’m tanned. American Journal of Health Be h av i or , 32, 243-252. Brandberg, Y., Ullen, H., Sjoberg, L., & Holm, L. E. (1998). Sunbath- ing and sunbed use related to self-image in a randomized sample of Swedish adolescents. European Journal of Cancer Prevention, 7, 321-329. doi:10.1097/00008469-199808000-00008 Broadstock, M., Borland, R., & Gason, R. (1992). Effects of suntan on judgements of healthiness and attractiveness by adolescents. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 157-172. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb01527.x Buss, D. M. (1994). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mat- ing. New York: Basic Books. Cafri, G., Thompson, J. K., Roehrig, M., Rojas, A., Sperry, S., Jacobsen, P. B., & Hillhouse, J. (2008). Appearance motives to tan and not tan: Evidence for validity and reliability of a new scale. Annals of Be- havioral Medicine, 35, 209-220. doi:10.1007/s12160-008-9022-2 Cokkinides, V. E., Weinstock, M. A., O’Connell, M. C., & Thun, M. J. (2002). Use of indoor tanning sunlamps by US youth, ages 11-18 years, and by their parent or guardian caregivers: Prevalence and correlates. Pediatrics, 109, 1124-1130. doi:10.1542/peds.109.6.1124 Forbes, G., Jobe, R., & Richardson, R. (2006). Associations between having a boyfriend and the body satisfaction and self-esteem of col- lege women: An extension of the Lin and Kulik hypothesis. Journal of Social Psychology, 146, 381-384. doi:10.3200/SOCP.146.3.381-384 Lamanna, L. M. (2004). College students’ knowledge and attitudes about cancer and perceived risks of developing skin cancer. Derma- tology Nursing, 16, 161-167. Leary, M. R., & Jones, J. L. (1993). The social psychology of tanning and sunscreen use: Self-presentational motives as a predictor of health risk. Journal of Applied Social Psychology , 23, 1390-1406. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1993.tb01039.x Lin, L. F., & Kulik, J. A. (2002). Social comparison and women’s body satisfaction. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 24, 115-123. Long, B. (1983). A steady boy friend: A step toward resolution of the intimacy crisis for American college women. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and App l i ed , 115, 275-280.
 T. F. PETTIJOHN II ET AL. 77 doi:10.1080/00223980.1983.9915445 Martin, J., Ghaferi, J., Cummins, D., Mamelak, A., Schmults, C., Parikh, M., & Liégeois, N. J. (2009). Changes in skin tanning atti- tudes: Fashion articles and advertisements in the early 20th century. American Journal of Public He a l th , 99, 2140-2146. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2008.144352 Mighall, R. (2008). I should Coco: As you prepare to ‘cover up’ on the beach this summer, lie back and enjoy Robert Mighall’s true history of sunbathing. History T o da y , 58, 31-33. Mosher, C. E., & Danoff-Burg, S. (2008). Social predictors of sun- screen and self-tanning product use. Journal of American College Health, 54, 166-168. doi:10.3200/JACH.54.3.166-168 Pettijohn, T. F. II, Pettijohn, T. F., & Geschke, K. S. (2009). Changes in sun tanning attitudes and behaviors of U.S. college students from 1995 to 2005. College S tu d e nt Jo u r nal, 43, 161-165. Reilly, A., & Rudd, N. A. (2008). Sun, salon, and cosmetic tanning: Predictors and motives. International Journal of Human and Social Sciences, 2, 170-176. Righton, B. (2005). Glow from a bottle. Maclean’s, 18, 40-41. Saad, G., & Peng, A. (2006). Applying Darwinian principles in design- ing effective intervention strategies: The case of sun tanning. Psy- chology & Marketing, 23, 617-638. doi:10.1002/mar.20149 Smith, K., Cornelissen, P., & Tovée, M. (2007). Color 3D bodies and judgements of human female attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 48-54. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.05.007 Stapleton, J., Turrisi, R., & Hillhouse, J. (2008). Peer crowd identifica- tion and indoor artificial UV tanning behavioral tendencies. Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 940-945. doi:10.1177/1359105308095068
|