 Creat ive Educati on 2014. Vo l.5, No.2, 75-85 Published Online February 2014 in SciRes (http://www.scirp.org/journal/ce) http://dx.doi. org/10.4236/ce.2014.52013 Religiosity, Social Support, Self-Control and Happiness as Moderating Factors of Physical Violence among Arab Adolescents in Israel Qutaiba Agbaria Educational Research and Development Authority, Al-Qasmie College, Baqa E l -Garbia, Israel Email: Q utaiba100psych@yahoo.com Received December 18th, 2013; revised January 18th, 2014; accepted January 25th, 2014 Copyrigh t © 2014 Qutaiba Agbaria. Thi s is an open access a rticle d ist ributed under t he Cr eati ve Common s At- tribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. In accordance of the Creative Commons Attribution License all Copyrights © 2014 are reserved for SC IRP and the owner of the intellectu al property Qutaiba Agbaria. All Copyright © 2014 are guarded by law and by SCIRP as a guardian. This study examines the correlation among a number of personal and environmental resources that can reduce violence among Arab adolescents in Israel. These are: religiosity, happiness, social support, and self-control . The par ticip ants in the study cons is ted of 2 25 Pa lesti nian Ara b teena gers l iving in Is rael par- ticipated in thi s study. The participants study in gra des 8 and 9, in s tate school s in the northern p art of The Tria ngle. The findi ngs indica te that al l the r esources t hat were examin ed contrib ute to reduci ng the level of viol ence; in ot her words, s ignifica nt negative corr elations were found between the le vel of reli giosity, happiness, social support and self-control on the one hand, and the level of violence on the other hand. These findings are consistent with those of other studies conducted elsewhere in the world on different populations (Christian and Jewish, as well as Muslim). The present study and its findings are, however, the fi rst to addr ess the unders tanding of violence amon g the pop ulace in ques tion. The f indings wer e dis- cussed in accordance with a number of different theor ies. Key words: Arab Adolesc ents; Violence; Reli giosit y; Happiness; Soci al Support; Self-Control Introduction Over recent years, violence in all its facets, and particularly physical violence, has become a common phenomenon in the State o f Israel, app earing in almost every enviro nment in which people live: in family and society, within the framework of stud ies and employment , on the road s, at recreatio n venues and on the sports field. One of the frameworks in which we are witness to many types of physical violence is the educational system, where both children and adults are affected (Ben-Baruch, 2005; Benbe- nishty, 2002). Although studies have pointed to a decrease in incidents of violence in schools between 1990-2000 (Benbe- nishty, Khoury-Kassabri, & Astor, 2006), studies undertaken in Israel among adolescents have shown that close to 40% of all Israeli students in public and high schools have reported en- countering many incidents of bullying and harassment at school (Benbenishty, 2002; Benbenishty, Zeira, & Astor, 2000; Rolid- er, 2007); more than 16% of the children reported that they had stayed away from school due to a fear that they would be ha- rassed, and others reported on great physical injury (Astor, Benbenishty, & Estrada, 2008). The issue of violence h as been the subject of extensive global research. Some studies have focused on the incidence, descrip- tion and scope of violence in society (Benbenishty, 2002; Ben- benishty et al., 2000; Rolider, 2007). Other studies have re- ferred to the phenomenon of violence in relation to gender (Ronen, R a ha v , & Moldaws ky , 2007; Warman & Cohe n, 2000). This present study focuses on the physical violence of Arab teenagers at s cho ol . Benbenishty et al.’s research (2006 ) reveals that more Arab students have reported on a violent atmosphere at school (49.8%) than the Jewish sector (40.6%). In addition, it was revealed that Jewish students have a better feeling about their schools coping with physical behavior than do Arab stu- dents (81.6% compared to 67.4% respectively); Jewish students also felt more of a kinship to their schools than Arab students (75.4% compared to 67.7% respectively). According to various studies undertaken in Israel (Benbe- nishty et al., 2006; Benbenishty & Astor, 2005), physical vi- olence and hooliganism at school were found to have a signifi- cant correlation with poverty, low educational levels and low socio -eco nomic status. According to data in the Bituach Leumi report on poverty for 2009, approximately 57% of Arab fami- lies are under the poverty line (Israeli National Insu rance Insti- tute, 2010). This data points to the existing liability among this popula tion to behave in a viole nt manner. Aside from the above, it is important to stress the signific- ance of th e maturat ion pro cess of Arabs in an Israeli society. As aforesaid, Arab teenagers live in a developing society which is undergoing an accelerated process of modernization that is linked to the Israelization process, on the one hand (Alhaj, 1996) and with counterparty processes of assimilation, expressed in Palestinization processes, on the other (Samuha, 2004). Inhe- rent in each of these processes is a system of different norms OPEN ACCE SS 75
Q. AGBARIA and valu es. Life in the shadow of these contradictions makes it difficult for these teenagers to consolidate their identities. There is a significant confusion between the adoption of a modern orientation influenced by Jewish society and the adherence to norms and values in Arabic tradition—I s l a m. In light of this, there is room for exa minat io n o f the degree, if an y, that western models referring to the phenomenon of physical violence in reference to Arab teenagers can be generalized and imple- mented. The core question of this study is: To what degree do meas- ures of religiosity, social support, well-being and skills of self- control moderate physical violence among Arab teen agers? Violence Violence has received a wide ran ge of definiti ons. The tradi- tional concept of violence has stressed the results of behavior and has defined violence as behavior whose results harm a per- son or property, where the harm may be psychological (dimi- nishing value and humiliation) or physical (Bandura, 1973). Despite this concept, the trend over the last decade has ac- tually been to stress physical behavior as being linked to the attacker’s in ten tion s, i.e. th e referen ce to vio len ce is to behavio r directed towards another with the intention of causing harm (Joireman, Anderson, & Stra thman, 200 3) . As with th e variet y of definitions, there have also been a va- riety of explanations for violent behavior. Traditional explana- tions have pointed to violence being an inherent instinct in everyone (Freud, 1924; Lorenz, 1966). In contrast, there are explanations that view violent behavior as being a result of frustration, where, when the individual feels frustration, the likelihood increases that violent behavior may be expressed (Dollard, Doob, Miller, Mowrer , & Sears, 1939) . The present study is based on the definition of Buss & Perry (1992), which corresponds to definitions of the cognitive beha- vioral approach, and sees physical violence as behavior linked to an individual’s misguided hostile thoughts and negative feelings (Bandura et al., 2001). A number of emphases on the problem’s development appear in this approach. Those based on social learning stress violent behavior being a social beha- vior acquired in the learning process via conditioning or imita- tion (Bandura, 1973), and linked to this is that undesired beha- vior, i .e. violence, has desi red results that reinforce it s continu- ation (Rolider, 2007). More modern explanations stress the cognit ive aspect of behavior (Bandura, Caprara, Barb aranelli, & Pastorelli, 2001), stressing violent behavior as stemming from the absence of the self-efficacy of the individual in a personal, academic and social sphere. Crick & Dodge (1996) propose a model of social learning and stress violent behavior as being behavior stemming from problems linked to the individual’s process of processing in- formation. According to their model, the process begins from the receipt of social cues and proceeds as far as the expression of violent behavior. It includes six stages: the selective encod- ing of internal and social cues that cause a violent child to re- ceive negative cues; the interpretation of these cues in a nega- tive and hostile manner; a reduction in one’s ability to seek out an appropriate response; lack of evaluation of so important a response in the case of violent behavior; making an incorrect decision that is not based on objective observation and proper evaluation; and action that leads to violent behavior. In accor- dance with this model, violent behavior will be linked to distor- tions in each of the stages in the processing of information (Dodge & Coie, 1987). Whether the explanation emphasizes more the result of un- desired behavior, the beliefs, the thoughts and processing of information or one’s feelings, according to the cognitive-beha- vioral approach—violent behavior is linked to the elements of thought, feelings and behavior together. In an attempt to examine the elements linked to violent be- havior, Buss & Perry (1992) have developed a model that ex- plains the tendency to develop violent behavior in accordance with the cognitive-behavioral model, with reference to the process of thought, feelings and behavior. The thinking process, i.e. the cognitive element, includes the hostile thoughts of an individual who conceives of the world around him as being hostile. This cognitive element is linked to negative feelings of anger, which constitute the emotional ele- ment in the trend towards violent behavior. According to the authors, hostile thoughts and negative feelings lead to the ex- pression of violent behavior, which include two parts: violent verbal behavior and violent physical behavior (Buss & Perry, 1992). With reference to the hostile-cognitive element, it is linked to continual negative thinking of the individual about his sur- roundings (Anderson & Bushman, 2002). This negative think- ing is characterized by a conception that the world is threaten- ing and unfair, that people act out of selfish motives and cannot be trusted (Buss, 1961). This element is express ed accordin g to the model for social information processing by Dodge and his colleagu es (Dodge, Laird , Lochman, & Zelli, 2002) in a hostile interpretation of the cues received by the individual from his surroundings. People with hostile thoughts will interpret situa- tions in a negative manner, and will tend more to assign hostile intentions to others and react with violence accordingly (Crick & Dodge, 1994; Hubbard, Dodge, Cillessen, & Coie S chwartz, 2001). A number of studies have indicated a correlation be- tween hostile thoughts and violent behavior among students (Bandura et al., 2001; Crick & Dodge, 1994; Hubbard et al., 2001). The emotional element, anger, is an emotional reaction ac- companied by physiological stimulation. Anger is stimulated by frustration, provocation and, often, anxiety (Buss, 1961). In addition, anger increases sensitivity to frustrating situations or obstacles, stimulates assertive thoughts and can encourage ac- tion against the threatening source (Cicchetti, Ackerman, & Izard, 1995). Anger is thought to be a predictor of a tendency for violent behavior (Arsenio, Cooperman, & Lover, 2000). The element of behavior includes both physical and verbal violen ce. Physical violence is defined as bodily harm of another with the objective of causing pain to the other (Buss, 1961). Verb al violen ce refers to a verb al message u tter ed to anot her, in an attempt to incur mental pain, and damage another’s posit ion and sel f-esteem (Infante, 1995) . The tool developed by Buss & Perry (1992) enables the ex- amination of violence as one parcel and to examine its overall score or separate it into different components (hostility, anger, verbal violence and physical violence), where each component earns a separate score that enables it to be examined as an ele- ment in itself. In the presen t stud y, we have decid ed to examine th e element of physical violence and the manner in which this violence is linked to hostile thoughts and feelings of anger, both due to our principal interest being in physical violence and the fact that the OPEN ACCE SS 76
Q. AGBARIA reliability of tools to examine verbal violence is poor (Weis- burd, Rosenbaum, & Ronen, 2009; Ronen & Devik, 2009; Ro- nen & Rosenbaum, 2010). Degree of Religio sit y Over recent years, we have been witness to a renewed awa- kening of the contribution of religion and spirituality to human behavior. Psychologists are beginning to recognize the idea that religion and spirituality can contribute to an improvement of wel l-being. Psychology has a traditionally negative position towards spirituality; indeed, psychologists and psychiatrists from Freud to Ellis have regarded religious orientation as being “irrational” and “a mechanism for people that are unable to cope with life” (Clay, 1996: p. 1). In spite of this, there has been an increase over recent years in studies that indicate the importance of religious faith in pro- viding meaning to life and in improving one’s wel l-being. Spi- rituality and religion may improve well-being (Witter, Stock, Okun, & Haring, 1985), self-esteem (Falbo & Shepperd, 1986), physical health (Gottlieb & Green, 1984) and satisfaction in married l ife (Glenn & Weaver , 1978) . The correlation between the degree of religiosity and mental health & psychopathology has prompted much research (Al- Issa, 2000; Maltby, Lewis & Day, 1999; Thorson, 1998). This correlation can be seen to be complex, where various explana- tions have been offered, such as “religiosity may repress the symptoms and encourage conventional forms of thought and behavior that are more socially accepted. This may provide sources for a wider development of perspectives and a fuller realization of individual capabilities” (Wulff, 1997: p. 244). Many studies have indicated a positive correlation between lack of spirituality and a number of negative psychological situations, such as depression (Wright, Frost & Wisecarver, 1993), drug use (Maton & Zimmerman, 1992) and anxiety and suicide (Gartner, Larson, & Allen, 1991). Ellison (1991) attempted to explain how spirituality and reli- giosity may promote happiness and well-being, in that religious observan ce (e.g. attend ing chur ch services) may in crease soci al integration and support. In addition, the development of a deep belief in God may promote happiness by reducing stress and improving strategies for coping. Moreover, religiosity and spi- rituality may provide meaning, solidarity and goals in life. It should be noted that the vast majority of research on this subject has been conducted in the west, with participants main- ly from Jewish and Christian populations. There have been few studies on Muslims and Arabs. This correlation has not been researched among Palestinian Arabs, and this study is consi- dered to be pioneering in the research on Arab Muslims and Christians who live in a Jewish country. Studies undertaken around the world on Muslim populations have indicated clear and positive correlations between religios- ity and a strong sense of well-being (Hussein, 1988; Abdel- Khalek, 2002, 2005; Alakandari, 2003; Suhail & Chaudhry, 2004), with less psychosomatic symptoms and more motivation (Hussein, 1998), with less anxiety (Abdel-Khalek, 2002) and with physical health and optimism (Abdel-Khalek & Naceur, 2007). One stu dy on t he corr elation b etween degree o f religio s- ity and violence has been undertaken by Watkins (2003), where the correlation between a high or low degree of religiosity and five measures of violence was studied. Results revealed that high grades in the degree of religiosity resulted in low grades in the degr ee of violence. Social Support Environmental resources include the individual’s social sup- port system (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). This system is attri- buted to all those individuals who have personal, social and familial relationships with others (Sarason, Sarason, & Pierce, 1990), and it refers to four major types of support: informative, instrumental, emotional and companionship support (Cohen & Wills, 1985). These t ypes of suppo rt make it easier for t he indi- vidual to cope with sources of stress (House, 1981). Professional literature presents two major forms of influence of social support on well-being. One form is the main effect, where social support has a direct positive effect on an individu- al’s well-being regardless of stress. The basic claim is that the support has the power to develop and increase feelings of abili- ty, self-esteem and self-ef ficacies. These feelings enable the individual to successfully cope with life’s challenges. The second form is the buffer effect, where social support has an indirect effect on an individual’s well-bei ng via the reduction of negative implications in the response to feelings of stress (An- tonucci & Akiyama, 1994; Cohen & Wills, 1985). In this man- ner, the support constitutes a coping strategy (Antonucci & Akiyama, 1994). Social support can moderate stress by affecting first or second impressions of an event (Cohen & Edwards, 1989). When the individual gains access to a support network that is available to him, it may suppress impressions of potential threat stemming from the event, encourage the individual to believe in his/her ability to cope with the event and/or encourage the use of opportunistic coping strategies, such as: problem-solving and positive re-evaluation (Cohen & Edwards, 1989; Cohen & McKay, 1984). Variou s studies add ress the sign ificance of social suppo rt for children and teenagers in situations of stress in general and situations of war in particular. For children experiencing a crisis, the famil y constitutes the main support system. During a crisis, the family provides its members with feedback on feelings, ideas and behavior. This determines the child’s understanding of the nature and significance of a stressful environment (Zeidner et al., 1993). Approximately 80% of Jewish students and approximately 64% of Arab students in this study turn to parents or family members during crises; approximately 25% of Jews and 50% of Arabs turn to their teacher or someone on the school staff; ap- proximately 16% of Jews and 38% of Arabs turn to school counselors, and approximately 75% of teenagers turn to friends when in distress (H ar el et al., 2004). Aside from family support, friends’ and teachers’ support also have a curbing effect on the psychological difficulties stemming from war (Klingman, 2001; Klingman et al., 1993; Swenson & Klingman, 1993; Zeidner et al., 1993). Studies by Greenbaum, Erlich, & Toubiana (1993), examining the utiliza- tion of sources of support among children during the Gulf War revealed that utilization of support originating from parents and friends is relatively higher than support originating at school and from telephone hot li ne s . While family support has greater importance during child- hood, friends and non-family members become a more signifi- cant source of support during teenage years (Cotterell, 1994). Most teenagers turn to friends their own age more th an to their OPEN ACCE SS 77
Q. AGBARIA parents for shared entertainment, friendship and understanding (Blyth, Hill, & Theil, 1982), and for feedback, practical infor- mation and emotional support (Jaffe, 1998). A peer group serves as a source for powerful social rewards, including pres- tige, acceptance, status and popularity, which promote a tee- nager’s self-esteem (Bishop & Inderbitzen, 1995). Relation- ships with one’s peers during teenage years are more characte- rized by intimacy and support than in early childhood (Jaffe, 1998); they play a crucial role in the promotion of normal psy- chological development (Steinberg, 2002) and constitute a pro- tective element during times of stress (Montemayor & Van Komen, 1980). Chen & Wei’s study (2013) examines how social support of peers mediates the correlation between school victimization and wel l-being among 1650 junior high school students in Taiwan, and later on examines how gender and ethnicity differ in reci- procal relations of violence at school, social support and well- being. Findings show that in general, students with a high level of well-being are not significantly linked with victimization by students in mediation of social support among peers. Another study by Benhorin & McMahon (2008) examines the effect o f social s uppo rt on the co rrelatio n between exp osure to violence and aggressive behavior. Findings demonstrate the negative effect of exposure to violence on aggressive behavior and the comprehensive contribution of social support in these relationships. Specifically, support from parents, teachers and close friends has been found to be positively linked to lower levels of aggressive behavior. Self-Control The term “self-control” describes behavior out of free choice, while relinquishing more appealing behavior for the sake of more desired behavior (Thoresen & Mahoney, 1974). This de- finition comprises two aspects: free choice, i.e. behavior that one chooses to express from the recognition that this behavior is important. This choice is not due to pressure from one’s ex- ternal surroundings or through lack of choice. And the second aspect–choice between two opposing behaviors, where one has a number of choices and must decide and select the more im- portant (or efficient) behavior over the more desired behavior at that particular moment (Ronen, 1997). Rosenbaum (1993, 1998) describes self-control as a system of cognitive, goal-oriented skills that enable people to act to attain their goals, to overcome difficulties linked to thoughts, feelings and behaviors; to delay gratification and cope with pressu re. According to Ronen & Rosenbaum (2001), the skill of self- control will be actuated only when the individual faces different obst acles that are d ifficult for h im to overco me and hamper t he attainment of his goals. This means that his goals and the ob- stacles in his way to attaining them are the ones that will deter- mine whether he will actuate his skills of self-control. There- fore, a teenager who does not see his aggressive behavior as being a problem and who may even obtain reinforcement fro m it, will not actuate his skills of self-control. In the present study, the use of the term “self-control skills” refers to the actuation of a group of skills to attain a desired goal. This group includes: cognitions and self-instructions for coping with various emotional and physiological responses, the use of strategies for problem-solving, the ability to delay grati- fication and the belief that one can control oneself during in- ternal events (Rosenbaum, 1980). In a number of studies conducted on children and t eenagers, it was found that those characterized with skills of self-control like delaying gratification, problem-solving and cognitive con- struction express less aggressive behavior (Blair et al., 2000; Ayduk et al., 2000; Denham, Kochanoff, & Whipple, 2004; Gyurak & Ayduk, 2008; Weisbrod, 2007). In addition, it was revealed that high levels of self-control are more linked with success in social relations, with more adaptive emotional res- ponses to pressure situations and with fewer reports on psy- chopathology (Tangney, Baumeister, & Boone, 2004). Studies by Agbaria, Ronen & Hamama (2012), examine the correlation between self-control and the incidence of psycho- pathological symptoms following the exposure of teenagers to war. Findings point to the moderating contribution of skills of self-control in the decrease of depression, anxiety and other psychopathological symptoms. In a study examinin g th e corr elatio n between sel f-control and anxiety/loneliness among siblings of children with cancer, a significant correlation was found between self-control as a coping skill and anxiety/loneliness as an emotional stress re- sponse; i.e., siblings reporting on higher levels of self-control experienced lower levels of anxiety and loneliness (Hamama, Ronen, & Fei gi n, 2009) . In another study examining the moderating effect of skills of self-control among women with a history of physical, sexual or emotional abuse, and with post-traumatic symptoms, it was found that women characterized by high levels of self-control reported on a lower intensity of post-traumatic signs in com- parison to women with low self-control (Walter, Gunstad, & Hobfoll, 2010). A study undertaken by Agbaria & Ronen (2010), focused on aggressive behavior among Palestinian Arab youths in Israel. 397 teenagers from grad es 7 to 12 part icipated in the study. One of the research theories addressed personal and social resources and examined the contribution of self- control and social acceptance in the reduction of violence; re- searchers found that skills of self-control will moderate the correlation between subjecti ve well-bei ng and violence. A study undertaken by Denson, Capper, Oaten, Friese & Schofield (2011), examined the belief that self-control over time might red uce anger an d violence as a res ponse to provoca- tion. Participants comprised B.A./B.Sc. students who under- went coaching on self-control for two weeks. Research data found that violence was reduced among participants with high levels of violent behavior, following coaching on self-control. Var ious studies support the claim that skills of self-control enable one to attain a balance between oneself, one’s environ- ment and one’s goals. Those who succeed in this balance suc- ceed in adapting themselves to environmental requirements and in coping efficiently with situations of pressure and stress (Rothbaum, Weisz, & Snyder, 1982). Happines s Happiness, defined by philosophers as exaltation, is very dif- ficult to pin down and define. Over recent decades, happiness has been the center of many studies, with renewed classification under more specific titles, such as subjective well-being, posi- tive effect and satisfaction fro m life (Di ener, 2009). Despite the resemblance between happiness and subjective wel l-being, most writers avoid the first term due to the variety of meanings to which it may refer (Diener, 2009). The most OPEN ACCE SS 78
Q. AGBARIA popular term in the field today—subjective well-being—refers to the individual’s subjective appreciation of his quality of life (Diener, 1984, 2009) and includes the elements of satisfaction with life (cognitive element) and the correlation between posi- tive and negative feelings (emotional element) (Diener, 2009). The term gains significance primarily in situations of great pressure and distress that may be detrimental to one’s well- being (Hobfoll, 1989; Natving, Albrektsen, & Quarnstrom, 2001; Torsheim & Wold, 2001). The term “subjective well-being” refers to the individual’s subjective appreciation of his quality of life, his happiness and gratification, as well as the quality of his inner experiences, with reference to various areas of life (Diener, 1984). Subjec- tive well-being includes elements like happiness, satisfaction, gratification and quality of life; these elements refer to cogni- tive and effective responses by an individual towards life expe- riences (Diener, 1984; Veenhoven, 1991). Those with a high sense of well-being feel more control over their lives and cope effectively with life’s pressures, setting themselves goals in life (Keyes & Ryff, 2000; Vee nhoven, 1991) . A number of studies have indicated positive correlations be- tween well-being and a sense of control over life (Veenhoven, 1991; McConnell et al., 2005), an ability to cope with pressures and conflicts (Argyle, 1987) and a tendency to exp erience fe w- er negative feelings (Fordyce, 1988). In addition, a positive correlation was found between social acceptance and physical and spiritual well-being (Bolger, Zuckerman, & Kessler, 2000; Spiegel , Bloom, Kraemer , & Gottheil , 1989). According to research literature, experiences with chronic and stressful life events affect the manner in which one eva- luates life satisfaction, as well as, correspondingly, with well- being (Ash & Huebner, 2001; Headey & Wearing, 1989), where there is a correlation between feelings of anxiety and stress experienced as a result of stressful life events (political, economic, social and familial) and a low sense of well-being (Campbell, 1981). Several studies indicate a negative correla- tion between life satisfaction and the development of post- traumatic symptoms among different populations, e.g. veteran soldiers (Merritt, Kashdan, Julian, & Uswatte, 2006), those with a background of chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) (Eglin- ton & Cheung Chung, 2011) and those with a background of drug use (Ouimette, Goodwin, & Brown, 2006). One stud y examined th e co rrelat ion bet ween bei ng victims of violence and feelings of well-being among teenagers. In this study, students from 190 high schools, ranging in age from 13 - 19, participated. Research results showed that boys and girls who were victims of violence reported on lower levels of life satisfaction (Cal lahan, Tolman, & Saunders, 2003). Another study focused on aggressive behavior among Pales- tinian Arab youths in Israel. In this study, 397 teenagers in grades 7 to 12 participated, and the correlation between well- being and violence was studied. Results showed a negative correlation between the two (Agbaria & Ronen, 2010). Hypot he sis There is a negative correlation between self-control and physical violence. There is a negat ive correlation b etween su pport and physical violen ce. There is a negative correlation between degree of religiosity and ph ysical violence. There is a negative correlation between well-being and physical violence. Method Sample 225 Pal estinian Arab teen agers livin g in Israel p articipat ed in this study. The participants study in grades 8 and 9, in state schools in the northern part of The Triangle. We know that in the northern part of the Triangle resides a Muslim population defined for the most part as a traditional society with interme- diate so cioeconomic stat us. It is important to note that the sam- pling method was convenience sampling. Approximately 80% of participants were grade 8 students, with approximately 20% in grade 9; 58.2% of participants were male and 41.8% were female. Tools Subjecti v e Happiness Scale: (Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1999) On the 4-item sub jective h appi ness scale, p articip ants cir cled the number that best characterized them on a 7-point Likert scale rangin g from 1 (ch aracterizi ng low levels o f happin ess) to 7 (characterizing high levels of happiness); for example, "In general I consider myself not a very happy person (1) to “a very happy person” (7) or “Compared to most of my peers I consider myself less happy” (1) to “very h appy” (7). 1) Attitudes Regarding Religion Questionnaire This questionnaire was developed by Kendler, Liu, Gardner, McCullough, Larson and Prescott, 2003. The q uest ionnai re was translated into Arabic and adapted to a Muslim Arab population; 5 items wer e deleted from the question naire and 12 items were added. The validity of the questionnaire was examined by 5 judges, and the pr eliminary format was corrected until the final format was attained. Cronbach’s alpha values for the different dimensions obtained were high and ranged from (α = 0.86 to 0.93). The questionnaire examines various Attitudes on religion and includes 63 items expressing 5 measures of positions on reli- gion: religiosity in general (spirituality), social religiosity, for- giveness, God as Judge and lack of desir e for revenge. Each participant was asked to evaluat e every one of the ite ms on a scale of 5 levels (1-never; 2-rarely; 3-sometimes; 4-oft en; 5-alway s). The present study made use only of the first dimen- sion which included 31 items, and which reflected more ele- ments of spirituality and understanding of one’s place in the universe, in addition to the contribution of the correlation with God as expressed in activities in daily life and during crises. 2) Adolescence Self Control Scale Questionnaire This questionnaire was originally developed by Rosenbaum (1980) in order to evaluate the individual differences in skills of self-control. The questionnaire examines self-reporting on the use of cognition (eg. self-instructions) and the implementation of problem-solving strategies to cope with emotional and phy- siological responses. The questionnaire was adapted for child- ren and t eenagers b y Rosen baum & Ronen (1991), and includes 32 items expressi ng d ifferen t p arameters in skill s of self-control: repressing gratification, getting over pain, planning abilities, use of self-instructions, etc. The participant would evaluate each one of the items on a six-level Likert scale, from (1-“very uncharacteristic of me” to 6-“very characteristic of me”). The questionnaire was reviewed according to a scale ranging from OPEN ACCE SS 79
 Q. AGBARIA (−3) to (3) points, representing the degree at which the partici- pant evaluates the item as being characteristic of him. The quest ionnaire contained 9 reverse items. In a test on qu estionn aire r eliab il ity with teen agers an d adult s (Rosenbaum, 1998), Cronbach’s alpha values for adults were found to be relatively high (0.87), where for children they wer e lower (0.69) (Hamama, 1996). In actuality, the older the child, the higher the alpha val ues. 3) Soc ial Support Questionnaire This questionnaire was evaluated using the Interpersonal Support Evaluate List (ISEL) developed by Cohen, Mermel- strin, Karmarck & Hoberman (1985), concerning the perceived availability of potential social resources. The original scale consists of 40 items, with 4 subscales (Appraisal, Belonging, Tangible Support and Self-esteem Support). The reliability of the ISEL is α = 0.90. In this study, we administered a brief version of this scale (12 items), including the first 3 subscales mentioned above, with 4 items in each. For example: “I feel that there is no one I can share my most private worries and fears with”. The items were rated on a 4-point scale ranging from (1) definitely false to (4) definitely true. Higher scores reflected greater perceived support. 4) Violence Q uestionnaire ( A GQ) This questionnaire was developed by Buss & Perry (1992), to evaluate the various elements in the trend towards violent behavio r among teen agers. The questionnaire contains 28 items testin g 4 element s of violence: physical violence (8 items), e.g. “If I am sufficiently provoked, I may hit another kid”; verbal violence (5 items), e.g.: “When other kids provoke me, I tell them what I think about them”; anger (7 items), e.g.: “Some- times I feel like I’m going to explode”; hostility (8 items), e.g.: “I know that other kids talk about me behind my back”. Each participant was asked to evaluate to what degree each of the items were ch aracteristic of him, vi a a 5-level Likert s cale, from (1) “very much disagree” to (5) “very much agr ee”. For each participant, a grade was calculated for each of the scales repr esenting th e elements of vio lence (ph ysical violence, verbal violence, anger and hostility). The grade was calculated as an average of all of the items included in the relevant sub- scale, takin g in to accou n t reverse i tems (3 revers e it e ms). In t he evaluation of questionnaire reliability in Hebrew, a Cronbach alpha val ue of α = 0.77 was found for “hostility”, α = 0.78 was found for anger, and α = 0.72 for verbal and physical violence (Zomer, 2002). Research Process The research questionnaires were approved by the Ministry of Education’s Chief Scientist, and then the questionnaires were distributed at designated schools. With the approval of school management, a letter was forwarded to parents explaining the objective of the research, and they were asked to approve or disapprove of their children’s participation in filling out the questionnaire. In the last stage, the primary researcher visited the schools on a regular study day. He entered the classes and explained the objective in filling out the questionnaires to the students, stressing that everything would be anonymous and that the findings would be utilized for research only. The level of participation was very high, with all students present agree- ing to fill out the questionnaire. The questionnaires were dis- tributed in Arabic (they had been translated at an earlier stage and uti lized for previous research). Findings Statist ical anal yses were cond ucted in order to ch eck correla- tions among the study’s variables and to test the research hy- potheses. Table 1 describ es Means, Standard Deviation, Range and Reliability of Research tools. As one can see from th e Table 2, clear, negative co rrelati ons were found between physical violence and the other variables (self-control, religiosity, happiness and social support); for examp le, there is a clear, negative correlation between self- contr ol and physical vio lence (r = −0.44, p < 0.01) and a nega- tive correlation between religiosity and physical violence (r = −.51, p < .01). To examine the research hypotheses, Pearson correlations were calculated and hierarchical regression analyses were con- ducted. The first hypothesis focused on the correlation between self control and physical violence, and hierarchical regression ana- lyses were conducted here as well. It was found that self control clearly contributes to decrease Physical Violence (BETA = −.14, p < 0.05). This confirmed the first hypothesis. The second hypothesis focused on the correlation between social support and Physical violence, and hierarchical regres- sion analyses were conducted here as well. In the hierarchical regression analysis that examined Physical violence as a de- pendent variable in relation to social support, it was found that it clearl y contri butes to the explan ation o f the varian ce in Phys- ical violence (BETA = −.23, p < .01). This confirmed the second hypothesis. See Table 3. Table 1. Means, standard deviation, range and reliability of research tools (N- 225). Variable Mean SD M edian Range Reliability Happiness 4.93 1.25 5.00 6.00 .59 Social Support 3.13 .73 3.16 3.33 .71 Religiosity 4.03 .94 4 .20 4.00 . 91 Self-control 3.88 .94 3.87 5.00 .81 Anger 2.81 .83 2.85 3.86 . 53 Physical violence 2.39 1.01 2.44 4.00 .81 Hostility 2.88 .85 2.83 3.83 .53 Verbal violence 2.9 7 .79 3.00 4.00 .50 Table 2. Correlations between the variables, based on Pears o n correlatio ns. Happiness 1 Social Support 2 Religiosity 3 Self Control 4 Physical Violence 5 1 --- 2 .62** --- 3 .45** .64** --- 4 .44** .45** .48** --- 5 −.47** −.54** −.51** −.44** --- Note: *p < .05; **p < .005; ***p < .001; N = 225. OPEN ACCE SS 80
 Q. AGBARIA Table 3. Regression Analysis of Physical violence. Beta Adjusted R-square Step 1 .11 Sex −.31** Grade .05 Step 2 .41 Self-control −.14* Social support −.23** Religiosity −.15* Happiness −.16* Notes: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. Sex: 0-ma le, 1-female; Grade: 0-grade 8, 1-grade 9. The third hypothesis focused on the correlation between reli- giosity and Physical violence. From Tabl e 2 (correlations), one can see that there is a clear, negative correlation between reli- giosity and Physical violence. In the hierarchical regression analysis, it was found that religiosity clearly contributes to the explanation of the vari ance i n P hysi cal vio len ce (BETA = −.15, p < .05). From here, we can conclude that the third hypothesis is con firmed. The fourth research hypothesis addressed the correlation be- tween happiness and Physical violence. Table 2 indicates a clear, negative correlation between happiness and Physical violen ce, a clear, posi tive correlatio n between negative feeli ngs and Physical violence. In the hierarchical regression analysis conducted on Physical violence, there was a clear contribution to the explanation of the variance in Physical violence (BETA = −.16, p < .05). From here, we can conclude that the research hypothesis was confirmed. This analysis found that sex contributes to explaining the va- riance in Physical violence. The contribution of gender was statistically significant. And found that among boys, the level of Physical violence is higher than among girls (β = −.31, p < .001). Discussion The present study was conducted among Arab junior high and high school students in Israel, and for the first time, ex- amined violence among Arab students, with reference to the variables of well-being, social support, religiosity and self- contr ol. In gen eral , one can say that th e presen t stud y’s findings support most of the theories. In addition, they correspond to and reinforce findings from previous studies obtained among other populations in Israel and across the world. For example, (Ronen, 2003, 2005; Baron, 2003; Ozden & Koksoy, 2009; Stuart & Holtzworth-Munroe, 20 05) . Self-Control The results of this study point to a clear, negative correlation between skills of self-control and Physical violence; i.e. it was found that among teenagers who exhibit skills of self-control, the level of Physical violence was lower, in addition to the me- diating effect of self-control on the correlation between expo- sure to Physical violence in the family and Physical violence. This finding is consistent with other studies in which a negative correlation was found between Physical violence and self-con- trol (Agbaria et al., 2013). A possible explanation for the finding is linked to the manner in which the skills of self-control are actioned , i.e. the abilit y of the teenager to identify automatic thoughts, to use diversions and alternative thinking, and to find alternative solutions which are skills of self-control, will lead him to choose controlled, planned, adaptive and less impulsive behavior. An additional explanation relies on Dodge et al.’s model for social informa- tion processing (Crick & Dodge, 1996; Dodge & Coie, 1987). According to this model, the ability to process information in a more balanced (less distorted) manner is dependent upon ap- plying th e appropriate in terpretation t o social and internal clues encoded by the individual. This interpretation will lead to a choice of controlled and adaptive behavior. The choice of this behavior rather than physically violent behavior is an expres- sion of the application of skills of self-control. Degree of Religiosity This study’s findings indicated a clear, negative correlation between religiosity and Physical violence. Research findings correspond to previous findings in research conducted on the subject (Koeing, McCullough, & Larson, 2001; Kaldestad, 1996; Thoresen & Harris, 2002; Khodayarifard, Ghobaribonab, & Shokohiyekta, 2000; Francis et al., 2004; Shehni, Yeylagh, Movahhed, & Shokrkon, 2002; Zimmeran, McDermut, & Mat- tia, 2000; Amrai, Zalani, Arfai, & Sharifian, 2011). This correlation between religiosity and Physical violence can be explained via various behavioral and cognitive ap- proaches. According to this approach, an individual’s behavior is a product of interpretations he applies to events he expe- riences. Therefore, every event that occurs obtains an interpre- tation that awakens different feelings, and these lead to beha- vior (Beck, 1995). From this explanation, we can conclude that religious people sustain beliefs and schema that provide them with a great deal of meanin g in life–meaning that makes it eas- ier for them to apply interpretations that will result in a calmer life management, and schema that can assist them in attaining self-control and well-being. In a preliminary qualitative study on a religious teenage population, the following schema arose: “In the end, God will help me”; “I don’t permit myself beha- viors that might anger God”; “Mo re acceptance an d patien ce in crises will earn me more divine rewards”, together with many schema based on chapters from the Koran or sayings by the prophet Muhammed, such as “Look for seventy excuses for your brother before you judge him”; “Strength is not seen through force but rather in holding back when angry”; “One must accep t all of God’s decrees” (Agbaria & Wattad, 2011). These schema render t ime an integr al portion of the believing person’s cognitive-behavioral repertoire and they will begin to influence and guide his behavior in various situations. In addi- tion to the behavioral portion of religious belief expressed in doing good deeds, prayers, visits to mosques, religious ceremo- nies, donations, classes and the social support represented in these activities, they can improve one’s spirit and increase a sense of belonging while providing meaning to life. Social Support This study’s findings reveal a clear, negative correlation OPEN ACCE SS 81
 Q. AGBARIA between social support and Physical violen ce. This finding is consistent with previous research in the field, where most report on a positive contribution to social support in an improvement in mental health and reduction in levels of violence (Irwin, LaGory, Ritchey, & Fitzpatrick; 2008; Marroquin, 2011) and suicidal thoughts (Galambos, Barker, & Krahn, 2006; Harris & Molock, 2000; Paykel, 1994; Stice, Ragan, & Randall, 2004; Talaei, F ayyazi, & Ardani, 2009; Travis, Lyness, Shields, King, & Cox, 2004). Social support is linked to an increase in the sense of one’s belon ging, where it is likely to d ecrease the feeling of pr essure through a response to the need for a bond with others via the distraction from worries linked to the situation or via alleviation in one’s mood (Cohen & Wills, 1985). Professional literature presents two principal types of effect of social support on well-being. One is the main effect, acco rd- ing to which social support has a direct positive effect on an individual’s well-being regardless of pressure situations. The basic claim is that support has the power to develop and in- crease feelings of ability, self-worth or self-efficacies. These feelings enable the individual to successfully cope with life’s challenges. The second type is the buffer effect, according to which social support has an indirect effect on the individual’s wel l-being, via the decrease of negative implications in re- sponse to feelings of pressure (Antoucci & Akiyama, 1994; Cohen & Wills, 1985). In this manner, social support consti- tutes a coping strategy (Antonucci & Akiyama, 1994). Social support can mediate pressu re by affectin g preliminar y or secondary assessments of an event (Cohen & Edwards, 1989). When the individual sees the support network as being available to him, it may suppress assessments of potential threats stemming fro m the even t; to encourage th e indi vidual to believe in his abilit y to cope with the even t and/or to encou rage use of adaptive coping strategies, such as: problem-solving and positive reassessment (Cohen & Edwards, 1989; Cohen & McKay, 1984). Therefore, and according to Lazarus & Folk- man’s model (1984), social support is conceived as an envi- ronmental resource that can assist in coping with pressure and difficult life situations. Well-Being Study results reveal a negative correlation between well-be- ing and violence. This finding adds to the knowledge accumu- lated in previous studies, which have pointed to an inverse cor- relation between well-being and violence among teenagers (Farrell & Bruce, 1997; Agbaria et al., 2013). A possible ex- planation for this finding is based on the significance found in the level of happiness in the acceleration of flexible thinking that leads to efficiency in problem-solving, to self-control, to thinking ahead, and to caution in risky situations (Aspinwall, 1998; Isen & Reeve, 2005). In other words, positive feelings contribute to less impulsive and more controlled and planned behavior. In addition, previous studies (Aresenio et al., 2000) reveal that positi ve feelings decrease fru stratio n and anxiet y, i.e. happy teenagers can better cope with frustration. They can de- velop social and interpersonal skills that serve as a defense against frus trating an d provocative events. Conclusion In conclusion, study findings point to the contribution of a number of resources in mediating violence among Arab tee- nagers. The research sheds light on the significance o f personal resources like self-control, religiosity and well-being, together with environmental resources like social support, in a decrease in the levels of violence. These study findings provide a theo- retical contribution to knowledge accumulated in reference to violence among teenagers and students, alongside a contribu- tion to its implementation, expressed in the development of coaching programs and programs on the use of skills that stress the importance of developing self-control, happiness and the developing of means to attain social support, together with an emphasis on the significance of attaining various religious val- ues to improve social behavior and decrease the tendency for violen ce. A number of factors limit the generalizability of this study. One of them is linked to the sampling. This study is based on a convenience sample that is not probabilistic and includes Mus- lim Arab teenagers from The Triangle only. The Triangle re- gion in Israel constitutes approximately 20% of the total Mus- lim Arab population. The sampling does not include representa- tion of Christians, Druze and Bedouin (Central Bureau of Sta- tistics, 2009) and therefore it is r ecommended t o undert ake this study on a more representative sampling in future. In addition, the present study utilizes self-report question- naires. These questionnaires provide the student’s perspective, but other elements, like groups of parents or peers may provide different information. In the present study, all study variables involve the teenager’s personal, internal elements. Thoughts and feelings cannot be evaluated via the teenager’s external environment. However, one must remember that the basis of self-reporting is limited and points to a tendency only. There- fore it is recommended in future to utilize additional research tools, such as questionnaires to teachers and peer groups. Another limitation linked to the aspect of self-report question- naires t ouches on social desirability. Continued studies may refine a portion of the findings in this study and may shed light on the relevant issues in this study, such as: economic status and study achievements. REFERENCES Abdel-Khalek, A. M. (2002). Why do we fear death? The construction and validati on of the rea sons for death fear scale. Death Studie s, 26, 669-680. http://dx.doi. or g/10.1080/074811802 90088365 Abdel-Khalek, A. M., & Naceur, F. (2007). Religiosity and its associa- tion with positive and negative emotions among college students from Algeria. Mental H ealth, Religion, and Cult ure, 10, 159-170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1369 4670500497197 Agbaria, Q. (2013). Depression among Arab students in Israel: The contribution of religiosity, happiness, s ocial support and self-control. Sociology Study, 3, 721-738. Agbaria, Q., & Ronen, T. (2010). Self control and a sense of social belongi ng as mod erators of th e link between p oor subj ective wellbe- ing and aggression, among Arab Palestinian adolescents in Israel. Social and Behavioral Sciences, 5, 1334-1345. Agbaria, Q., Ronen, T., & Hamama, L. (2012). The link between deve- lopmental components (age and gender), need to belong and re- sources of self-control and feelings of happiness, and frequency of symptoms. Children and Youth Services R ev iew, 34, 2018-20 27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2012.03.009 Agbaria, Q., & Watad, N. (2011). Religiosity and self-cont rol as related ment a l welfa r e of s t ud en t s. 3rd I nternational Virtu al Confe r ence, Tel Aviv. Al-Hajj, M. A. (1996). Identity and orientation among the Arabs in Israel. St ate, Governm ent and Int ernat ional Rela tions, Hai fa Univer- OPEN ACCE SS 82
 Q. AGBARIA sity. Al-Issa, I. (2000). Al-Junun: Mental illness in the Islamic world. Madi- son, CT: International Universities Press, Inc. Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 27-51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231 Antonucci, T. C., & Akiyama, H. (1994). Convoys of attachment and social relations in children, adolescents and adults. In F. Nestmann, & K. Hurrer lmann (Eds.) , Social networks and social support in childhood and adolescence (pp. 37-52). New York: Walter d e Gru y- ter. Argyle, M. (1987). The psychology of happiness. London: Methuen. Arsenio, W. F., Cooperman, S., & Lover, A. (2000). Affective predic- tors of preschoolers’ aggression and peer acceptance: Direct and in- direct effec ts. Developmental Psyc hology , 36, 438-448. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012 -1649.36.4.438 Ash, C., & Huebn er, E. S. (20 01). Environ mental even ts and li fe sati s- faction reports of adolescents: A test of cognitive mediation. School Psychology International, 22, 320-336. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0143 034301223008 Astor, R. A., Benbenishty, R., & Est rada, J . (20 08). Sch ool violenc e. In T. Mizrahi & L. E. Davis (Eds.), The encyclopedia of social work. Washington, DC: NASW Press. Ayduk, O., Mendoza-Denton, R., Mischel, W., Downey, G., Peake, P. K., & R odrigu ez, M. (2000). Regu lating the i nterpers onal s elf: Str a- tegic self-regulation for coping with rejection sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 776-792. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022 -3514.79.5.776 Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ : Prentice-Hall. Bandura, A., Caprara, V., Barbaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., & Regalia, C. (2001). Socio cognit ive self regulatory mech anisms governin g tran s- gressive b ehavi or. Journal o f Pe rsona lity a nd So c ial Psy cholo gy, 80, 125-135. http://dx.doi.or g/10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.125 Benbenishty, R. (2002). Violence in th e school syst em—Interim report on the findings of a sur vey among students. http://www.education.gov.il/scientist/alimut.htm Benbenishty, R., & Astor, R. A. (2005). School violence in context: Culture, neighborhood, family, school, and gender. New York: Ox- ford Uni versi ty Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/97 80195157802.001.0001 Benbenishty, R., Zeira, A., & Ast or, A. (2000). Violence in education: Summary report for 1999-1998. Jerusalem: Hebrew University. Benbenishty, R., Khoury-Kassabri, M., & Astor, R. (2006). Research Group: Mental health and well-being of Children and adolescents. Jeru salem: Hebrew Uni v ersity, School of S ocial Work. Beck, U. (1995). Ecological politics in the age of risk. Cambridge: Polity Press. Benhorin, S., & McMahon, S. D. (2008). Exposure to violence and aggression: Protective roles of social support among urban African American youth. Journal of Community Psychology, 36, 723-743. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jcop.20252 Bishop, J., & Inderbitzen, H. (1995). Peer acceptance and friendship: An investigation of their relation to self-esteem. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 15, 476-489. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0272 431695015004005 Blair, K., Denham, S., Kochanoff, A., & Whipple, B. (2004). Playing it cool: Temperament, emotion regulation, and social behavior in pre- schoolers. Journal of School Psychology, 42, 419-443. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2004.10.002 Bl yth , D. A., Hi ll, J. P., & Thei l , K. (1982). Early adolescents’ signifi- cant others: Grade and gender differences in perceived relationships with familial and non-familial adults and young people. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 11, 425-45 0. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/B F01538805 Bolger, N., Zuckerman, A., & Kessler, R. C. (2000). Invisible support and adjustment to stress. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 79, 953-961. http ://dx.d oi.org/10.1037/0022 -3514.79.6.953 Buss, A., & Perry, M . (19 92). Th e aggression qu estionna ire. Jou rnal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 452-459. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022 -3514.63.3.452 Cal lah an, M. R ., Tolma n. R. M., & Saunders, D. G. (2003). Adolescent dating violence victimization and psychological well-being. Journal of Adolesc en t Res e arch, 18, 664-681. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0743 558403254784 Chen, J. K., & Wei, H. S. (2013). School violence, social support and psychological health among Taiwanese junior high school students. Child Abuse & Neglect, 37, 252-262. htt p://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.01.001 Cicchetti, D., Ackerman, B. P., & Izard, C. E. (1995). Emotions and emotion regulation in developmen tal psychopat hology. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 1-10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400006301 Coh en, L. H., Het tler, T. R. , & Pa rk, C. L. (199 7). Soci al suppor t, per- sonality, and life stress adjustment. In G. R. Pierce, B. Lakey, I. G. Sarason, & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Sourcebook of social support and per son ality (pp. 215-228). New York: Plenum. Cohen, S., & Edwards, J. R. (1989). Personality characteristics as mo- derators of the relationship between stress and disorder. In R. W. J. Neufeld (Ed.), Advances in the investigation of psychological stress (pp. 235 -283). New York: Wiley. Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffer- ing hypothe s is . Ps yc hological Bulletin , 98, 310-357. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033 -2909.98.2.310 Cohen, S., & McKay, G. (1984). Social support, stress and the buffer- ing hypothesis: A theoretical analysis. In A. Baum, S. E. Taylor, & J. E. Singer (Ed s.), Handbook of psycholo gy and he alth (pp. 253-267). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cotterell, J. (1994). Analyzing the strength of supportive ties in adoles- cent social suppor ts. In F. Nest m ann, & K. Hurrelmann (Eds.), Social netwo rks and soc ial suppo rt in childh ood and adol escence (pp. 257- 267). Berlin: Walter de Gru yter. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/9783 110866377.257 Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms on reactive and pr oactive a ggression . Child Development, 67, 993-1002 . http://d x.doi.org/10.2307 /1131875 Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjust- ment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74-101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033 -2909.115.1.74 Davis, T. L., Kerr, B. A., & Robinson Kurpius, S. E. (2003). Mea ning, purpose, and religiosity in at-risk youth: The relationship between an- xiety and spirituality. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 31, 356 - 365. Denson, T. F., Capper, M. M., Oaten, M., Friese, M., & Schofield, T. P. (2011). Self-control tra inin g dec rea ses aggression in resp onse to p ro- vocation in aggressive individuals. Journal of Research in Persona- lity, 45, 252 -256. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.001 Diener, E. (2009). The science of well-being—The collected works of Ed Diener. Dordrecht: Springer. Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542-575. http://dx.doi.org/10 .1037/0033 -2909.95.3.542 Diener, E., & Clifton, D. (2002). Life satisfaction and religiosity in broad probability samples. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 206-209. Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social information-processin g factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. Spe- cial issue: Integrating personality and social psychology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1146-115 8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.6.1146 Dodge, K. A., Laird, R., Lochman, J. E., & Zelli, A. (2002). Multidi- mensional latent-construct analysis of children’s social information process ing patterns : Correlation s with aggressive b ehavior prob lems. Psychological Asse ssment, 14 , 60-73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040 -3590.14.1.60 Dollard, J., Doob, L. W., Miller, N. E., Mowrer, O. H., & Sears, R. R. (1939). Frustration and aggression. New Haven, CT: Yale Univer- sity Press. http://dx.doi.or g/10.1037/ 10022-000 Ellis on , C. G. (1991). Religi ous involvemen t and subj ecti ve well-being. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 32, 80-99. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2136 801 OPEN ACCE SS 83
 Q. AGBARIA Falbo, T., & Shcppcrd, J. A. (1986). Self-righteousness: Cognitive, power and religious characteristics. Journal of Research in Persona- lity, 20, 145 -157. http://dx.doi. org/10.1016/0092-6566(86)90114-5 Fife, B. L. (2005). The role of constructed meaning in adaptation to the onset of life-threatening illness. Social Science and Medicine, 61, 2132-2143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.soc s cimed.2005.04.026 Fo rd yce , M . W. (1988). A review of research on the happiness measures: A si xty second index of happiness and mental health. Social Indica- tors Rese arch, 20, 355-381. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/B F00302333 Gartner, J., Larson, D., & Allen, G. (1991) . Religious c ommitment and mental health: A review of the empirical literature. Journal of Psycho- logy a nd Theology, 19, 6-25. Glenn, N. D., & Weaver, C. N. (1978). A multi-variat, multi-survey study of marital happiness. Journal of Marriag e and the Family, 40 , 269-282. Gottlieb, N. H., & Green, L. W. (1984). Life events, social network, life-style, and health: An analysis of the 1979 national survey of per- sonal health practices and consequences. Health Education & Beha- vior, 11, 91 -105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1 177/109019818401100105 Green bau m, C., Erli ch, C., & Tou biana , Y. (199 3). Set tler chi ldren and the Gulf War. In N. A. Fo x, & L. A. Leavitt (Eds.), The p sychologi- cal effects of war and violence on child ren (pp. 109-130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Gyurak, A., & Ayduk, O. (2008). Resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia buffers against rejection sensitivity via emotion control. Emotion, 8, 458-467. http:/ /dx.doi. org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.458 Harris, J. I., Schoneman, S. W., & Carrera, S. R. (2002). Approaches to religiosity related to anxiety among college students. Mental Heal th, Religion, and Culture, 5, 253-265. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1367 4670110112730 Headey, B. W., & Wearing, A. J. (1989). Personality, life events, and s ub- jective well-being: Towards a dynamic equilibrium model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 731-739. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022 -3514.57.4.731 House, J. S. (1981). Work stress and social support. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Hubbard, J. A., Dodg e, K. A., Cillessen, A. H. N., Coi e, J. D., & Sch wa rtz, D. (2001). The dyadic nature of social information processing in boys’ reactive and proactive aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 268-280. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022 -3514.80.2.268 Infante, D. A. (1995). Teaching students to understand and control ver- bal a ggr ession. Communicatio n Education, 4 4, 51-63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0363 4529509378997 Jaffe, K. (1 998). Sex, mate selection and evolution. In V. W. Porto, N. Saravanan, D. Waagen, & A. E. Eiben (Eds.), Lecture notes in com- puter science 1447: Evolutionary programming VII (pp. 483-492). Berlin: Spring er Verlag. Joireman, J., Anderson, J., & Strathman, A. (2003). The aggression paradox: Understanding links among aggression, sensation seeking, and the consideration of future consequences. Journal o f P ersonal ity and Social Psychology, 84, 1287-1302. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022 -3514.84.6.1287 Keyes, C. L. M., & Ryff, C. D. (2000). Subjective change and mental healt h: A self-concept theory. Social Psychology Quarterly, 63, 264- 279. http ://dx.doi .org/10.2307/2695873 Klingman, A, Sagi, A., & Ra viv, A. (1993). The effect of war on Israeli children. In L. Leavitt, & N. Fox (Eds.), The psychological effects of war and violence on children (pp. 75-92). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Klingman, A. (2001). Israeli children’s reactions to the assassination of the prime minister. Death Studies, 25, 33-49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0748 1180125937 Koenig, H. G., McCullough, M. E., & Larson, D. B. (2001). Handbook of rel i gion and h e alth. New York: Oxford Universi ty Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/97 80195118667.001.0001 Landau, S. F., Bjorkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K. M. J., Osterman, K., & Gideon, L. (2002). The effect of religiosity and ethnic origin on di- rect and indirect aggression among males and females: Some Israeli findings. Aggr essive Beha vior, 28, 281-298. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.80006 Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and coping. New York: Springer-Verlog. Levin, J. S., & Cha tters , L. M. (1998 ). Researc h on religi on and ment al health: An overview of empirical findings and theoretical issues. In H. G. Koenig (Ed.), Handbook of religion and mental health (pp. 33-50). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B97 8-012417645-4/50070-5 Lorenz, K. (1966). On agg ression (Wil s on, M. K., Trans.). New Y ork : Harcourt, Brace & World, Inc. Maltby, J., Lewis, C. A., & Day, L. (1999). Religious orientation and the psychological well-being: The role of the frequency of personal prayer. Britis h Journa l of Healt h P sycho logy, 4, 363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/1359 10799168704 Maton, K. I., & Zimmerman, M. A. (1992 ). Psychosoci al predictors of substance use among urban black male adolescents. Drugs and So- ciety, 6, 79-114. http://d x.doi.org/1 0.1300/J 023v06n01_04 Montemayor, R., & Van Komen, R. (1980). Age segregation of ado- lescents in and out of school. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 9, 371-381. http://dx.d oi.org/10.10 07/BF02087675 Natvig, G. K., Albrektsen, G., & Qvarnstrom, U. (2001). School-related stress experience as a risk factor for bullying behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 30, 561 -575. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:10 10448604838 Pearlin, L. I. (1985). Social structure and processes of social support. In S. Cohen, & S. L. Syme (Eds.), Social support and health (pp. 43-60). New York: Academic Press. Ronen, T., Rahav, G., & Moldavsky, A. (2007). Aggressive behavior among Israeli elementary school students and associated emotional/ behavioral problems and self-control. School Psychology Quarterly, 22, 407-431. http ://dx.d oi.org/10.10 37/1045-3830.22.3.407 Ronen, T., & Rosenbaum, M. (2010). Developing learned resourcefulness in adolescents to help them reduce their aggressive behavior: Preli- minary findings. Research on So c ial Work Pr actice, 20, 410-426. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1049 731509331875 Ronen, T., & Rosenbaum, M. (2001). Helping children to help them- selves: A case study of enuresis and nail biting. Research on Social Work Practice, 11, 338-356. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1049 73150101100304 Rosenbaum, M. (1998). Opening versus closing strategies in contr olling one’s responses to experience. In M. Kofta, G. Weary, & G. Sedek (Eds.), Personal control in action: Cognitive and motivational me- chanisms (pp. 61-84). New York: Plenum Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978 -1-4757-2901-6_3 Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J. R., & Snyder, S. S. (1982). Changing the world and changing the self: A two process model of perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 5-37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022 -3514.42.1.5 Samoha, S. (2004). Index of Arab-Jewish relations in Israel 2004. Haifa: Th e Jewish-Arab C en ter. (in Hebrew) Sandler, I. N., Miller, P., Short, J., & Wolchik, S. A. (1989). Social support as a protective fact or for children in stress. In D. Belle (Ed.), Children’s social networks and social supports (pp. 277-307). New York: John Wiley. Sarason, B. R., Sarason, I. G., & Pierce, G. R. (1990). Traditional views of soci al supp ort and thei r impact on assess ment . In B. R. Saras on , & I. G. Sarason (Eds.), Social supp ort: An in teracti onal view (pp. 9-25). Oxford : Wiley. Spiegel, D., Bloom, J. R., Kraemer, H. C., & Gottheil, E. (1989). Effect of psychosocial treatment on survival of patients with metastatic breast cancer. Lancet, 334, 888-891. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S014 0 -6736(89)91551-1 Steinberg, D. M. (2002). The magic of mutual aid. Social Work with Groups, 25, 31-38. Suhail, K., & Chaudhry, H. R. (2004). Predictors of subjective well- being in an eastern Muslim culture. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, 359-376. Swenson, C. C., & Klingman, A. (1993). Children and war. In C. F. Saylor (Ed.), Children and disasters (pp. 137-163). New York: Ple- num Press. http://d x.doi.org/10.1007/9 78-1-4757-4766-9_9 Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self OPEN ACCE SS 84
 Q. AGBARIA control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersona l success. Journal of Personality, 72, 271-324. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-3506.20 04.00263. x Thoresen, C. E., & Mahoney, M. J. (1974). Behavioral self-con trol. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Veenhoven, R. (1991). Is happiness relative? Social Indicators Research, 24, 1-34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF0 0292648 Walter, K. H., Gunstad, J., & Hobfoll, S. E. (2010). Self-control pre- dicts later symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Pra ctice an d P olicy, 2, 97-101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018958 Warman, D. M., & Cohen, R. (2000). Stabili ty of aggressi ve behavior s and children’s peer relationships. Aggressive Beh avior, 26, 277-290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1098 -2337(2000)26:4<277::AID-AB1>3.0. CO;2 -C Watkins, S. J. (2003). Religiosity and aggression in college students. Electron ic Th eses and D isser tations, Paper 799. http://dc.etsu.edu/etd/799 Weisburd, B., Rosenbaum, M., & Ronen, A. (2009). Sensitivity to rejection and aggression among adolescent and young adolescents. Social and We lfare, 1, 107-131. (in He bre w) Witter, R. A., Stock, W. A., Okun, M. A., & Haring, M. J. (1985). Religion and subjecti ve well-being in adulthood: A quantitative syn- thesis. Review of Religious Research, 26, 332-342. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3511 048 Wright, L. S., Frost, C. J., & Wisecarver, S. J. (1993). Church atten- dance, meaningfulness of religion, and depressive symptomatology amon g adolesc ents. Journal of You th and Ad olescence, 2 2, 559-568. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/B F01537716 Wulff, D. M. (1997). Psychology of religion: Classic and contemporary (2nd ed. ). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Zeidner, M., Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1993). Hebrew adapta- tion of the general self-efficacy scale. Health Psychology, 12, 102 - 104. OPEN ACCE SS 85
|