Psychology
2013. Vol.4, No.12, 1027-1029
Published Online December 2013 in SciRes (http://www.scirp.org/journal/psych) http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2013.412149
Open Access 1027
The Impact of Criminal Code Training on Eyewitness
Identification Accuracy
Michael Storozuk1, Paul Dupuis2
1Department of Criminology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
2Department of Psychology, Algoma University, Sault Ste Marie, Canada
Email: paul.dupuis@algomau.ca
Received August 16th, 2013; r evised September 14th, 2013; accepted October 13th, 2013
Copyright © 2013 Michael Storozuk, Paul Dupuis. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative
Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly cited. In accordance of the Creative Commons Attribution License all
Copyrights © 2013 are reserved for SCIRP and the owner of the intellectual property Michael Storozuk, Paul
Dupuis. All Copyright © 2013 are guarded by law and by SCIRP as a guardian.
Eyewitness identification accuracy of offenders (persons who committed a crime) is generally unreliable.
In this study, we implemented a training approach to examine the impact of a brief criminal law training
session on the identification accuracy of eyewitnesses viewing a simulated violent altercation between
two males. Participants provided with prior training on how to appropriately apply specific criminal law
definitions relevant to a violent altercation (assault and self-defense provisions) were more accurate in
their identifications of the offender when compared to participants provided with irrelevant training (a riot
and the unlawful assembly of a riot), and participants provided with no training, when observing the same
violent altercation. Potential implications and limitations are discussed.
Keywords: Eyewitness Identification; Criminal Law; Training
Introduction
One of the most prevalent findings in the forensic psychol-
ogy literature is that eyewitness identification is often unreli-
able and inaccurate (Leippe, Eisenstandt, & Rauch, 2009; Web-
ber & Perfect, 2012). This finding is largely stable across a
variety of crime types (Wells & Olson, 2003; Clark, Marshall,
& Rosenthal, 2009) as well as across a variety of lineup proce-
dures and techniques (see Steblay, Dysart, & Wells, 2011;
Wells & Olson, 2003 for reviews). As many of these estimator
and system variables have been shown to contribute to unreli-
able and inaccurate eyewitness memory, we were interested in
assessing eyewitness identification accuracy from a different
perspective, a training perspective. Specifically, we were inter-
ested in examining whether or not training in certain sections of
the Criminal Code would impact eyewitness identification ac-
curacy for a dyadic violent crime altercation, a social interac-
tion that leads to the exchange of criminal violence between
two people (Athens, 2005).
Eyewitness offender identification accuracy for dyadic vio-
lent crime altercations can be particularly unreliable, in large
part because the processing of information about the offender
can be difficult for an eyewitness (Deffenbacher, Bornstein,
Penrod, & McGorty, 2004; Marsh & Greenberg, 2006). With
both individuals (i.e., offender and victim) of a dyadic violent
crime altercation exchanging acts of criminal violence (Athens,
1997; Guerette & Santana, 2010), there is a potential decrease
in the amount of attention an eyewitness can devote to the of-
fender’s characteristics. A lack of attention may hinder the
eyewitness’s memory for the offender’s characteristics, and
consequently negatively impact identification accuracy in the
event that they are later questioned by the police (Tuckey &
Brewer, 2003; Hellman, Echterhoff, Hopietz, Niemeier, &
Memon, 2011).
Our intention in this study is to provide an empirical ap-
proach that can be offered as a potential starting point for future
research. This approach involves training in specific aspects of
criminal law. Criminal laws are in the public domain and the
average citizen is expected to have some knowledge of these
laws. For example, one of the main purposes of criminal laws
(e.g. the Canadian Criminal Code) is to inform the public of the
actions they are criminally prohibited from performing (Her-
mida, 2011). An implication for an eyewitness is that if they
notice that an incident involves a crime and can determine
which behaviors in a given incident are criminal, their attention
can be devoted more appropriately to relevant details of the
incident (e.g. the offender). However, the assumption that the
average person is knowledgeable enough about criminal law to
apply this to an incident involving criminal behavior is tenuous.
Therefore, we investigated whether training eyewitnesses
about how to appropriately apply the content within a given
violent crime definition (how to identify the behaviors impli-
cated in the criminal act) and a given justification (how to iden-
tify who is and who is not justified in committing such a crimi-
nal act) would increase their ability to accurately identify the
offender of a dyadic violent altercation, when compared to
eyewitnesses who did not receive such training. We hypothe-
sized that eyewitness identification of the offender would be
more accurate for participants in the specific-training group
than for participants in the nonspecific-training group and the
control group. Our hypothesis was based on the assumption that
involvement in the specific-training group would disambiguate
M. STOROZUK, P. DUPUIS
the dyadic violent crime altercation in regards to discerning
between the offender and victim. We thought that this could
increase participants’ awareness of the offender’s characteris-
tics when they observed the altercation, increase participants’
memory for the offender, and therefore result in more accurate
eyewitness identifications.
Methods
We presented participants (78 introductory psychology stu-
dents from Algoma University) with a fifty-eight second vide-
otaped violent altercation depicting two males committing
criminal violence as per the assault definition in the Criminal
Code (see section 265 of Criminal Code, 1985). As per the
self-defence definition in the Criminal Code (see section 35 of
Criminal Code, 1985), one of the males was justified in
self-defence (i.e., the victim) and the other was not (i.e., the
offender). Between five and seven days prior to viewing the
violent altercation and performing the eyewitness identification
procedure, participants randomly received one of three forms of
training. Participants were either trained how to appropriately
apply the assault and self-defence definitions to a hypothetical
case (the specific-training group), how to appropriately apply
an irrelevant Criminal Code definition (riot and unlawful as-
sembly of a riot) to a hypothetical case (the non-specific train-
ing group), or received no training (the control group). Follow-
ing the training and the presentation of the videotaped violent
altercation, participants were all presented with a six person,
target present, simultaneous lineup and asked to identify the
person from the video who committed assault or to indicate that
the offender was not present. The lineup consisted of the of-
fender and five foils (known innocents not present in the video).
The victim from the video never appeared in the lineup.
Results
The impact of group training (i.e., specific-training group,
non-specific training group, and control group) on eyewitness
identification accuracy of the offender was analyzed using a
One-Way ANOVA. All statistical analyses were conducted at
the p < 0.05 significance level. There was a statistically sig-
nificant main effect for the impact of group training. Results
indicated that eyewitness identification of the offender was
most accurate for participants in the specific-training group (M
= 1.30), less accurate for participants in the control group (M =
1.60), and least accurate for participants in the non-specific
training group (M = 1.78) (F (2, 75) = 7.34, p < 0.01). Post-hoc
analyses revealed that eyewitness identification was more ac-
curate for participants in the specific-training group than for
participants in the non-specific training group (p < 0.01) or the
control group (p < 0.05). The identification accuracy of par-
ticipants in the control group did not differ from those in the
non-specific training group (p > 0.05).
Discussion
Research shows that eyewitnesses are generally unreliable
when it comes to identifying an offender from a crime they
observed (Wells & Olson, 2003; Webber & Perfect, 2012).
Consequently, a long standing problem faced by researchers
has been the question of how to improve eyewitness identifica-
tion accuracy. Researchers have explored numerous variables to
gauge the impact on eyewitness identification accuracy with
generally limited success (see Wells & Olson, 2003).
Ambiguity over explicitly distinguishing between the of-
fender and victim may negatively impact an eyewitness’s in-
formation processing of the offender’s characteristics when
they are observing a dyadic violent altercation, thereby poten-
tially negatively impacting an eyewitness’s identification accu-
racy (e.g., Tuckey & Brewer, 2003; Hellman, Echterhoff,
Hopietz, Niemeier, & Memon, 2011). We thought that a poten-
tial approach to improving eyewitness offender identification in
regard to this ambiguity issue would be to provide eyewitnesses
with prior training on how to appropriately apply Criminal
Code definitions that are directly relevant to the behavior of the
individuals (i.e., offender and victim) who are party to a given
dyadic violent altercation.
The experimental results supported our hypothesis that par-
ticipants given specific training on relevant Criminal Code
sections would be more accurate in identifying the offender.
Results indicated that eyewitness identification was more accu-
rate for participants in the specific-training group than for par-
ticipants in the non-specific training and no training groups. At
face value, this finding could imply that providing laypersons
with Criminal Code training may be a useful approach to take
toward improving eyewitness identification accuracy of dyadic
violent altercations. Further research is needed to determine if
specific training of this sort consistently leads to higher identi-
fication accuracy as well as how this might be occurring. Per-
haps the attention and focus of eyewitnesses were directed
more appropriately when given specific training due to a de-
crease in the ambiguity of the incident. This can only be im-
plied from our findings and further empirical research is needed
to directly address this potential explanation. Further research
could also explore whether this type of criminal law training
could be successful for other types of crimes. For example,
would specific training in criminal negligence promote accurate
eyewitness identifications for participants exposed to a simu-
lated case involving multiple people only one of which was
criminally negligible? Additionally, introductory psychology
students do not necessarily represent the average citizen well.
Would conducting similar research within a broader participant
pool find similar results? Future research could also examine
whether having different gender or ethnicity combinations of
offenders and victims would result in similar findings? Our data
cannot speak to these questions, but perhaps can encourage
more directed attempts to address these and other related issues.
REFERENCES
Athens, L. (1997). Violent criminal acts and actors revisited. Chicago:
University of Illinois Press.
Athens, L. (2005). Violent encounters: Violent engagements, skirmi-
shes, and tiffs. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 34, 631-678.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0891241605280570
Clark, S. E., Marshall, T.E., & Rosenthal, R. (2009). Lineup adminis-
trator influences on eyewitness identification decisions. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: A ppli ed, 15, 63-75.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015185
Criminal Code, R.S.C. C-46 (1985).
laws-lois.justice.gc.ca/eng/acts/C-46/
Deffenbacher, K. A., Bornstein, B. H., Penrod, S. D., & McGorty, E. K.
(2004). A meta-analytic review of eyewitness memory. Law and
Human Behavior, 28, 687-706.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10979-004-0565-x
Open Access
1028
M. STOROZUK, P. DUPUIS
Open Access 1029
Guerette, R. T., & Santana, S. A . (20 10). Explaining victim self-protec-
tive behavior effects on crime incident outcomes: A test of opportu-
nity theory. Crime & Delinquency, 56, 198-226.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0011128707311644
Hellman, J. H., Echterhoff, G., Kopietz, R., Niemeier, S., & Me mon, A .
(2011). Talking about visually perceived events: Communication ef-
fects on eyewitness memory. European Journal of Social Psychology,
41, 658-671.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.796
Hermida, J. (2011). Criminal law. The Netherlands: Kluwe Law Inter-
national.
Leippe, M. R., Eisenstandt, D., & Rauch, S. M (2009). Cueingconfi-
dence in eyewitness identifications: Influence of biased lineup in-
structions and pre-identification memory feedback under varying li-
neup condition. Law and Human Behavior, 33, 194-212.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10979-008-9135-y
Marsh, D. P., & Greenberg, M. S. (2006). The influence of eyewitness
similarity to a crime victim and victim culpability on eyewitness re-
call. Applied Psychology in Criminal Justice, 2, 43-56.
Steblay, N. K., Dysart, J. E., & Wells, G. L. (2011). Seventy-two tests
of the sequential lineup superiority effect: A meta-analytic analysis
and policy discussion. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 17, 99-
139. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021650
Tuckey, M. R., & Brewer, N. (2003). The influence of schemas, stimu-
lus ambiguity, and interview schedule on eye witness memory over
time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 9, 101-118.
Webber, N., & Perfect, T. J. (2012). Improving eyewitness identifica-
tion accuracy by screening out those who say they don’t know. Law
and Human Behavior, 36, 28-36.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0093976
Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (2003). Eyewitness testimony. Annual
Review of Psychology, 54, 277-295.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145028