Psychology
2013. Vol.4, No.11, 858-863
Published Online November 2013 in SciRes (http://www.scirp.org/journal/psych) http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2013.411123
Open Access
858
Perception of Partner Abuse and Its Impact on Marital Violence
from Both Spouses
Claude Bélanger1,2, Cynthia Mathieu1, Hélène Brisebois1
1Department of Psychology, University of Quebec in Montréal, Montreal, Canada
2Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
Email: belanger.claude@uqam.ca
Received August 27th, 2013; revised September 28th, 2013; accepted October 27th, 2013
Copyright © 2013 Claude Bélanger et al. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons
Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the
original work is properly cited.
Few studies have investigated bi-directional models of marital violence. Research suggests that female
victims are also often perpetrators of violence. Accordingly, some researchers propose that we should test
the hypothesis that the victim and perpetrator roles can be played by both men and women. The current
study addresses this issue by attempting to understand the effect that perceptions of spousal violence will
have on both partners’ level of marital violence. Our objectives were to verify the links between levels of
violence and perceptions of violence by both partners, and actual self-reports of each type of violence
perpetrated. We verified if self-reports and partner’s reports of violence would differ, if one partner’s
abuses would influence the other partner’s abuses, and whether the spouse’s self-reported violence or the
other spouse’s perception of that violence had a differential impact on the level of violence perpetrated.
Twenty-three couples in which the male partner was undergoing treatment for marital violence took part
in the study. Results indicate that for both partners perceptions of partner violence modulate the level of
marital violence that is perpetrated. The link between perceptions and violent behaviors appears to explain
female marital violence better than that it does for males. Implications based on these results are discussed.
Keywords: Marital Violence; Psychological Violence; Physical Violence; Spousal Abuse; Perception of
Abuse
Introduction
Domestic violence has been a growing concern for research-
ers and clinicians since more than 50 years. This important
societal problem has been recognized by the American Psy-
chiatric Association planning and research committees who is
considering inclusion of a Marital Abuse Disorder diagnosis
(Marital Conflict Disorder with Violence) in the new Relational
disorders of the forthcoming DSM-5 (2013). Numerous studies
have looked at different predictors and models that would ex-
plain and predict marital violence. A significant amount of re-
search that has been published on marital violence is studying
male perpetrators and female victims (McQueen, 2013). This
might be explained by the fact that females are more likely to
report spousal abuse than men, with the consequence that fe-
male violence might be underreported by men. Some authors
would take a different position and support a bi-directional mo-
del of marital violence, and a growing number of studies have
gone beyond the traditional model, with results indicating that
spousal abuse is also perpetrated by women Some researchers
concluded that a significant proportion of females seeking help
for victimisation are also perpetrators of intimate partner vio-
lence (Williams, Ghandour, & Kub, 2008). In their review arti-
cle on domestic violence, McNeely and Mann (1990) state that
“classifying spousal violence as a women’s issue rather than a
human issue is erroneous”. They propose that we should look
beyond the traditional ways of investigating marital violence
and try to understand and explain the problem keeping in mind
that the victim and perpetrator roles can be played by both men
and women.
One of the first researchers to identify marital violence
against men found an equal amount of violent acts on the part
of both partners (Gelles, 1974). Later, Gelles and Straus (1988)
conducted a comparative study of the National Family Violence
Surveys of 1975 and 1985. In their widely read paper, they
stated that 11.6% of husbands reported experiencing abuse from
their wives within the previous year and 4.6% reported being
victims of severe violence (use of a knife or gun, kicking, beat-
ing, punching, etc.). The same author carried a more recent re-
search (Strauss, 2001) where he studied prevalence of marital
violence among students of 31 Universities worldwide. He re-
ported that males and females had very similar rates of physical
assaults toward their partners, even for severe assaults (25% of
men and 28% of women for physical assaults, and 9% for se-
vere assaults for both genders). In a study of twenty-three dating
couples, Archer and Ray (1989) found that women were more
likely than men to use physical violence towards their partner.
In an investigation conducted in New Zealand by Magdol, Mof-
fit, Fagan, Newman and Silva (1997), 37.2% of women and
21.8% of men reported being victims of physical violence in
the previous year, whereas 18.6% of women and 5.7% of men
reported being victims of severe violence. Other studies have
made similar findings and established similar patterns of spou-
C. BÉLANGER ET AL.
sal abuse from both genders (Cascardi, Langhinrichsen, & Vi-
vian, 1992; Mason & Blankenship, 1987; Margolin, 1987). Deal
and Wampler (1986) found that 47% of their university student
based sample had been victims of intimate violence. Most of
the abuse was reported to be reciprocal and when it was not,
men were three times more likely to report being the victims.
Fiebert and Gonzalez (1997) studied a sample of college wo-
men in the United States to better understand women who initi-
ate assault of their male partner. Twenty-nine percent of the
women indicated they had initiated marital violence in the past
five years; the two main motives were (a) because they did not
think that their spouse would retaliate or would be injured and
(b) as an attempt to draw emotional attention from them. La-
fontaine and Lussier (2002) support this bidirectional model of
intimate violence. They reported that both men and women
were victims of physical and psychological violence.
Archer (2000) conducted a meta-analytic study on gender
differences in marital aggression and found that even if women
were more likely than their male partners to use physical ag-
gression, they were also more likely to sustain injuries resulting
from their partner’s violence. This would suggest that although
women may be physically abusing their partners, the latter’s
physical abuse is more likely to injure them. If the impact of
violence is different between genders, it would also be possible
that the perception and interpretation of the abuse from the
other spouse would be different between males and females.
According to a study conducted in 1991 by Follingstad, Wright
and Sebastian, men interpret their partner’s use of violence as a
means of showing their anger or retaliation for feeling emo-
tionally hurt or mistreated, while women describe their part-
ner’s intentions as a desire to gain control over them or as a
retaliation for being assaulted first. In a study of college cou-
ples, Matthews (1984) found that both men and women, whe-
ther they were perpetrators or victims, would often interpret
marital violent acts as a form of love. Holtzworth-Munroe and
Hutchinson (1993) found that violent husbands are more likely
to attribute blame, negative intents and selfish motivations to
their wives. O’Leary and Vivian (1990) hypothesized that attri-
butions can mediate aggressive behavior towards a spouse.
Previous studies have focused on attributions and variables
underlying male and female marital violence. To our knowl-
edge, no authors have studied the impact of perception of spou-
sal violence on marital violence in a bi-directional model. An
examination of both partners’ perception of their mate’s vio-
lence and of how it is linked to both partners’ marital violence
would be both innovative and fit nicely with previous studies of
attributions as predictors of violence within the marriage. Som-
me issues in that matter would be interesting to explore. For
instance, would self-reports and partner reports of violence be
significantly different? Does one partner’s abuse influence the
other partner’s level of physical and psychological violence and
sexual coercion? If so, is it the one partner’s actual violence or
the other partner’s perception of that violence that modulates
the level of male and female marital violence? The present stu-
dy addresses these questions and seeks to determine if there is a
link between both spouses’ perception of the other spouse’s
violence on one hand, and their levels of marital violence on the
other hand. More precisely this study will try to better under-
stand the relationship between the perception of the other
spouse’s violence on one hand and levels of marital violence on
the other hand. It would also be of interest to know if attribu-
tions (Bradbury & Ficham, 1990; Ficham, 1994) or the judg-
ment on the part of one partner as to why the other partner re-
sorts to violence in their exchanges, would differ for men and
women.
Objectives
Our objectives were to verify the links between levels of
violence and perceptions of violence by both partners, and ac-
tual self-reports of each type of violence perpetrated. More spe-
cifically, our objectives were to test 1) if self-reports and part-
ner’s reports of violence would differ significantly, 2) if one
partner’s abuses would influence the other partner’s abuses, and
3) whether the spouse’s self-reported violence or the other
spouse’s perception of that violence had a differential impact
on the level of violence perpetrated.
Method
Participants
Our sample consisted of twenty-three couples where the male
partner was undergoing treatment for physical domestic vio-
lence. The mean level of education for the women and men was
11.6 years and 11.3 years, respectively, ranging from less than a
high school diploma (high school diploma = 11 years) to a uni-
versity degree (16 years). The mean age for the women and
men was 30.3 years and 34 years, respectively. All the couples
had children, with an average of 1.9 children per couple. In
terms of annual income, 57.1% of the women earned $14,999
or less, 19.1% earned between $15,000 and $29,999, 19.1%
earned between $30,000 and $44,999, none of the women earn-
ed between $45,000 and $59,999, and 4.7% earned between
$60,000 and $74,999. As for their male partners, 19% earned
$14,999 or less, 19% earned between $15,000 and $29,999,
23.8% earned between $30,000 and $44,999, 28.6% earned
between $45,000 and $59,999 and 9.5% earned between
$60,000 and $74,999. The couples were not necessarily living
together at the time the male partner was receiving treatment.
Fifteen percent of the couples had limited contact, 5% saw each
other once a week, 15% saw each other several times a week,
and 65% were still in a full-time relationship.
Procedure
The couples in the sample were recruited at a community
center that offered treatment for violent men in the Montreal
region (Canada). Both partners completed questionnaires that
included both self- and partner reports of violence. Spouses
were instructed not to discuss their answers with the other part-
ner.
The intervention consisted of two pre-therapy meetings and
25 weekly group therapy sessions. Therapists presented the stu-
dy to the men at the first meeting and if they agreed to par-
ticipate they filled out the questionnaires after the second pre-
therapy meeting. Men were informed that their decision to par-
ticipate in the study would not influence their therapeutic proc-
ess in any way and that they were free to withdraw from the re-
search project at any time. Questionnaires with a preaddressed
envelope were also mailed to their partners. To ensure confi-
dentiality and to prevent the men from exerting any kind of
pressure on their partners, we informed the female partners that
they were free to participate or not in our study and that their
Open Access 859
C. BÉLANGER ET AL.
decision would not be divulged to their partner nor would it
affect the therapy that their partner was undergoing.
Instruments
A French version of the Revised Conflict Tactics Scales
(CTS2) originally developed by Straus et al. (1996) and trans-
lated and validated by Lussier (1997) was used to evaluate four
different types of violent behavior: psychological violence,
physical violence, sexual coercion, and injuries resulting from
violence. In each couple, both spouses completed independ-
ently a questionnaire that included a self-report of their own
violence and their perception of their spouse’s violence for the
four forms of violence examined. All items were rated accord-
ing to the frequency of each violent behavior with eight differ-
ent categories. Scores were calculated as follows: Except for
scores of 0, 1 or 2 (for which the score is the same as the mid-
dle point), it is obtained by addition of middle-points. When the
participant answered 3 (3 to 5 times), the middle point was 4,
when they answered 4 (6 to 10 times), the middle point was 8,
for the answer 5 (11 to 20 times), the middle point was 15 and
finally, for the category 6 (more than 20 times) the authors re-
commended to use 25 as the middle point. For the French ver-
sion, alpha coefficients are acceptable and range from .70 to .79,
except for physical violence, which has an alpha of .46. Alpha
coefficients for the English version range from .79 to .95, with
psychological violence having the lowest score.
Results
Means and standard deviations for the violence subscales for
men and women are presented on Table 1.
Correlational analyses were conducted between the women’s
different types of violence and their perception of their spouse’s
violence. Results: see Table 2.
Women’s use of psychological violence appears to be linked
to their perception of their spouse’s psychological violence. As
for women’s physical violence, it is linked to their perception
of their spouse’s psychological and physical violence and use
of sexual coercion, and their perception of the number of inju-
ries they had sustained as a result of spousal abuse. The number
of injuries that women inflicted on their husbands was linked to
their perception of their spouse’s physical violence and their
perception of the number of physical wounds they had received
as a result of spousal abuse.
We conducted correlational analyses between men’s different
types of violence and their perception of their partner’s vio-
lence. Results are presented in Table 3.
It would appear that the only link between men’s violence
and their perception of their spouse’s violence is the number of
physical wounds inflicted on their spouse, which is in turn po-
sitively related to the number of wounds inflicted on them by
the women.
Correlational analyses were conducted between women’s
self-report of violence and men’s reports of their partner’s vio-
lence. Results are presented in Table 4. Results indicate that the
only form of men’s violence as reported by both partners that
seems to be correlated are the injuries inflicted to women by
men.
We finally conducted correlational analyses between men’s
self-reports of violence and women’s reports of their spouse’s
violence. Results are presented in Table 5.
Table 1.
Means and standard deviations on the different subscales of violence
for men and women.
Psy. viol Phys. viol CoercionInjuries
inflicted
’s self-reports 6.05 (4.4)1.15 (2.2) 34 (.7) 57 (1.2)
’s reports of
’s viol 8.21 (6.2)2.20 (3.8) 1.84 (2.8).99 (2.1)
’s self-reports 3.61 (3.8).69 (1.3) .025 (.09).18 (.4)
’s reports of
’s viol 3.63 (3.3).71 (1.1) 18 (.5) .33 (.9)
Note: Psych. viol = psychological violence; Phys. viol = men’s physical violence;
Coercion = men’s use of sexual coercion; Injuries inflicted = Injuries inflicted on
their spouse.
Table 2.
Links between women’s violence and their perceptions of their
spouse’s violence.
Psych.
viol. Phys. viol. Coercion Injuries
inflicted
Psych. viol. .63, p = .002
Phys. viol. . 56, p = .007.82, p = .000 .45, p = .035 .75, p = .000
Injuries
inflicted .45, p = .03 .75, p = .000
Note: Psych. viol. = women’s psychological violence; Phys. viol. = women’s
physical violence; Injuries inflicted = Injuries inflicted by the women on their
spouse; Psych. viol = men’s psychological violence (as reported by women);
Phys. viol = men’s physical violence (as reported by women); Coercion =
men’s use of sexual coercion (as reported by women); Injuries inflicted = In-
juries inflicted by the men on their spouse (as reported by women).
Table 3.
Correlations between men’s violence and their perceptions of their
spouse’s violence.
Injuries
Injuries .631 p = .001
Note: Injuries = Injuries inflicted by men on their spouse; Injuries = Injuries
inflicted by women on their spouse (as reported by the men).
Table 4.
Correlations between men’s self-reported violence and men’s violence
as reported by their spouse.
Injuries inflicted by
Injuries inflicted by () .425 p = .043
Note: Injuries inflicted by = injuries inflicted by the men on their spouse; In-
juries inflicted by () = injuries inflicted by the men on their spouse (as re-
ported by women).
Men and women seem to agree to a greater degree on their
reports of different forms of women’s violence. Their reports of
women’s use of psychological and physical violence and of
sexual coercion seem to be correlated.
Paired sample t-tests were then carried out to detect any sta-
tistically significant differences between the men’s and women’s
reports of violence. To accomplish this, we compared the wo-
men’s self-reports of violence to women’s violence as reported
by the men. We then compared the men’s self-reports of vio-
Open Access
860
C. BÉLANGER ET AL.
Table 5.
Correlations between women’s self-reported violence and women’s
violence as reported by their spouse.
Psych. viol. Phys. viol. Sex. coercion
Psych. viol. () 586 p = .003
Phys. viol. () .474 p = .022
Sex. coercion () .586 p = .003
Note: Psych. viol. = psychological violence perpetrated by women (self-re-
ports); Psych viol. () = psychological violence perpetrated by women (as re-
ported by men); Phys. viol. = physical violence perpetrated by women (self-
reports); Phys. viol. () = physical violence perpetrated by women (as reported
by men); Sex. coercion = Sexual coercion used by women (self-reports); Sex.
coercion () = Sexual coercion used by women (as reported by men).
lence to the men’s violence as reported by the women. Results
showed that for women’s psychological violence, physical vio-
lence and sexual coercion, the women’s self-report numbers
were higher than the men’s report of the women’s violence.
However, the mean differences were not statistically significant.
As for reports of injuries inflicted by the men, the men’s self
reports tended to be significantly higher than the women’s re-
ports of the men’s violence.
Discussion
This study aimed at a better understanding of the role of per-
ceptions of partner’s violence as a regulator of both men and
women’s intimate violence. Results in regard to the differences
in self-reports and partner’s reports of violence, as well as the
impact of perceptions on both partners’ violence will be dis-
cussed.
Our results show that there is often a relationship between
men’s and women’s reports of their spouse’s violence and self-
reports of their own violence. Although there were no statisti-
cally significant differences, men tended to under-report their
partner’s violence. This observation would detail with a report
from O’Keefe (1997) who postulated that men may be reluctant
to report violence perpetrated by their mate because they fear
they would get a negative reaction from their peers.
In the present study, a significant difference was found be-
tween men’s and women’s reports of their partner’s violence
was with respect to injuries: compared to men’s self-reports of
injuries inflicted on their spouses, women tended to under-re-
port the number of injuries received in the past year. Women
may tend to underestimate the number of injuries they have re-
ceived from their partners. This could possibly be interpreted
by the fact that, if these women are still with their partners, they
need to diminish their cognitive dissonance related to the fact
that they are in a relationship with someone who hurts them.
As for the impact of perceptions on the various forms of vio-
lence perpetrated by women, their perception of their partner’s
violence was linked to their own violent behaviors. As wo-
men’s perception of the level of their spouse’s psychological
abuse increased, their own psychological abuse increased. As
their perception of their husband’s psychological violence, phy-
sical violence, use of sexual coercion, and infliction of injuries
increased, there was an increase in their own physical violence.
Lastly, as women’s perception of their spouse’s physical vio-
lence and infliction of injuries increased, the level of injuries
that they inflicted on their husbands increased.
For men, the only perception that appeared to be related to
their violence was that the number of injuries they inflicted in-
creased as the number of injuries they reported their spouse had
inflicted on them in the past year increased.
It would thus appear that women’s perceptions of their part-
ner’s violence provided a better explanation of their own vio-
lence than the actual levels of abuse perpetrated by their partner.
This is an interesting finding because it portrays interpretations
of marital violence as being different for men and women. Per-
ceptions of spousal abuse may be an important component in
our understanding of female violence. Interpreting female vio-
lence as self-defense (Pleck et al., 1978) does not appear to
apply to the models of intimate violence proposed by most
researchers, but it would be interesting to investigate further to
learn why the notion of self-defense was thought to be the only
reason underlying female violence. Verifying if abusive women
perceived a threat prior to using violent behavior would give us
an indication of why they plead self-defense, and if this is the
case it would be interesting to work with them on perceptions
of threat and actual signs of danger. It would appear that wo-
men who are in a relationship with a violent man report more
violent acts by their spouse than the latter do. Perhaps, as
O’Keefe (1997) indicated, men are hesitant to report being
victimized in their close relationship because they are afraid of
not being taken seriously. Also, it is important to differentiate
the women of our sample from women in the general popula-
tion who are in a violent relationship. It is possible that the
women in our sample have already identified the fact that they
are in a violent relationship and have hence taken action to deal
with this problem. Their spouse being in treatment usually
means either that they were arrested for marital violence, most
often because the wife had called the police, or that they were
“strongly” advised by their spouse to enter a treatment program.
Our results need to take into account this idiosyncracy and thus
cannot be generalized to all women in violent relationships.
Women also seem to perpetrate more violent acts when they
perceive violence on the part of their partner. Are these wo-
men’s perceptions altered by the fact that they are in a relation-
ship with a violent man? Are women excusing their own vio-
lence by associating it with perceptions of their husband’s vio-
lence? Why is perception of spousal abuse a more significant
variable in explaining female violence than it is for men? Per-
haps women are more inclined to listen to their feelings or
emotions before acting out violence. Their feelings of fear may
alter the perception they have of their spouse’s violence and
provide them with the incentive they need to defend themselves.
Lafontaine and Lussier (2005) reported a link between insecure
attachment and anger, and marital violence. There may be a re-
lation between a woman’s fear and type of attachment, and her
level of violence towards her partner. Further studies need to
explore these questions by examining the link between emo-
tions and perpetration of violence in both men and women.
Additionally, these studies could analyse the link between per-
ceptions of the partner’s violence and other marital variables
such as marital adjustment and attachment and (Gosselin, La-
fontaine, & Bélanger, 2005).
Limitations of the Present Study
The present study has some limitations. First, since the num-
ber of couples (n = 23) is relatively small, it is possible that our
sample is not representative of couples in the general popula-
Open Access 861
C. BÉLANGER ET AL.
tion where the man is identified as being violent. The coup les
in our study were recruited in a facility offering group counsel-
ing for violent men; perhaps not all violent men seeking help
are interested in this form of therapy. Wealthier and more edu-
cated men may opt for private counseling or other forms of help
they can afford. A second limitation is the fact that we relied on
CTS2 reports and did not measure each partner’s attributions of
their spouse’s violence. The CTS2 does not take into account
the context in which the violence takes place. It is thus very
difficult to determine which partner initiated the violence. Per-
haps men or women in our sample under-reported levels of vi-
olence for social desirability reasons, which we did not measure.
Further studies could investigate links between perceptions of
spousal violence and violent acts with other measures that are
specifically designed to interpret perceptions of intimate vio-
lence.
Differences between self-reported violence and violence as
reported by the spouse for both partners were presented. Our
findings provide a new look at the impact of perception of
spousal violence on the level of marital violence of both
spouses. Clearly, it would be beneficial for future research to
investigate bi-directional marital violence in other populations
such as couples seeking help for therapy, couples from the gen-
eral population or couples in which the man has been violent
but who is not interested in seeking help for his problem. This
research could be replicated with the addition of a questionnaire
which would permit to identify the instigator of violence. It
would also be of interest to measure perceptions of violence.
Further research should aim at differentiating women and
men’s intimate violence by adding interpretations of violent be-
haviors from the partner, in addition to his/her own perceptions
of violence, as a complement to actual violent behaviors. This
distinction could be crucial to increase efficacy in treating these
individuals, because false perceptions and attributions are cen-
tral in the communicational dysfunctions and emotional dysre-
gulation that characterize these couples.
Acknowledgements
This publication was funded by a Canadian grant from the
Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC) to
the first author.
REFERENCES
Archer, J., & Ray, N. (1989). Dating violence in the United Kingdom:
A preliminary study. Aggressive Behavior, 15, 337-343.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1098-2337(1989)15:5<337::AID-AB24801
50502>3.0.CO;2-0
Archer, J. (2000). Sex differences in aggression between heterosexual
partners: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 651-
680. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.5.651
Bradbury, T. N., & Ficham, F. D. (1995). Attribution in marriage. Psy-
chological Bulletin, 1 0 7 , 3-33.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.1.3
Cascardi, M., Langhinrichsen, J., & Vivian, D. (1992). Marital aggres-
sion; Impact, injury, and health correlates for husbands and wives.
Archives of internal Medicin e , 152, 1178-1184.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archinte.1992.00400180048007
Deal, J. E., & Wampler, K. S. (1986). Dating violence: The primacy of
previous experience. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 3,
457-471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407586034004
DeMaris, A. (1992). Male versus female initiation of aggression: The
case of courtship violence. In E. C. Viano (Ed.), Intimate violence:
Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 111-120). Bristol, PA: Taylor &
Francis.
Ficham, F. D. (1994). Cognition in marriage. Applied and Preventive
Psychology, 3, 185-198.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0962-1849(05)80070-8
Fiebert, M. S., & Gonzalez, D. M. (1997). Women who initiate assaults:
The reasons offered for such behavior. Psychological Reports, 80,
583-590. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1997.80.2.583
Follingstad, D. R., Wright, S., & Sebastian, J. A. (1990). Sex differ-
ences in motivations and effects in dating violence. Family Relations,
40, 51-57. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/585658
Gelles, R. J. (1974). The violent home: A study of physical aggression
between husbands and wives. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.
Gosselin, M., Lafontaine, M. F., & Bélanger, C. (2005). L’impact de
1’attachement sur la violence conjugale: L’état de la question. Bul-
letin de Psychologie, 58, 579-588.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/bupsy.479.0579
Lafontaine, M. F. (2002). Are typologies of psychological and physical
couple violence linked to romantic attachment and dyadic adjustment?
Unpublished Theses, Trois-Rivieres: University of Quebec.
Lafontaine, M. F., & Lussier, Y. (2005). Does anger towards the par-
tner mediate and moderate the link between romantic attachment and
intimate violence. Journal of Family Violence, 2 0 , 349-361.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10896-005-7797-5
Lussier (1997). A French translation of the revised conflict tactics
scales. Unpublished Manuscript, Trois-Rivieres: University of Quebec.
Mangdol, L., Moffitt, T. E., Fagan, J., Newman, D. L., & Silva, P. A.
(1997). Gender differences in partner violence in a birth cohort of 21
year olds: Bridging the gap between clinical and epidemiological ap-
proaches. Journal of Consulting and cl i nical Psychology, 65, 68-78.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.65.1.68
Margolin, G. (1987). The multiple forms of aggressiveness between
marital partners: How do we identify them? Journal of Marital and
Family Therapy, 13, 77-84.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1752-0606.1987.tb00684.x
Marshall, L. L., & Rose, P. (1987). Premarital violence: The impact of
the family of origin violence, stress and reciprocity. Violence and
victims, 5, 51-64.
Mason, A., & Blankenship, V. (1987). Power and affiliation motivation,
stress and abuse in intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 52, 203-210.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.52.1.203
Matthews, W. J. (1984). Violence in college couples. College Student
Journal, 18, 150-158.
McNeely, R. L., & Mann, C. R. (1990). Domestic violence is a human
issue. Journal of Interp e r s o nal Violence, 5, 129-132.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/088626090005001012
McQueen, D. (2011). Violence is most commonly reciprocal. The Psy-
chiatrist, 35, 33.
O’Keefe, M. (1997). Predictors of dating violence among high school
students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 12, 546-568.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/088626097012004005
Pleck, E., Pleck, J. H., Grossman, M., & Bart, P. B. (1978). The bat-
tered data syndrome: A comment on Steinmetz’s article. Victimology:
An International Journal, 2, 680-683.
Straus, M. A., & Gelles, R. J. (1986). Societal change and change in
family violence from 1975 to 1985 as revealed by two national sur-
veys. Journal of Marriage and th e Family, 48, 465-479.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/352033
Straus, M. A. (1995). Trends in cultural norms and rates of partner vi-
olence: An update to 1992. In S. M. Stitch, & M. A. Straus (Eds.),
Understanding partner violence: Prevalence, causes, consequences,
and solutions (pp. 30-33).
Strauss, M. (2001). Prevalence of violence against dating partners by
male and female university students worldwide. Violence against
Women, 10, 790-811. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1077801204265552
Williams, J. R., Ghandour, R. M., & Kub, J. E. (2008). Female perpe-
tration of violence in heterosexual intimate relationships. Trauma
Violence Abuse, 9, 227.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1524838008324418
Open Access
862
C. BÉLANGER ET AL.
Open Access 863
Whitaker, D. J., Haileyesus, T., Swahn, M., & Saltzman, L. S. (2007).
Differences in frequency of violence and reported injury between re-
lationships with reciprocal and nonreciprocal intimate partner vio-
lence. American Journal Pu blic Health, 97, 941-947.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2005.079020