J. Biomedical Science and Engineering, 2010, 3, 1133-1142
doi:10.4236/jbise.2010.312147 Published Online December 2010 (http://www.SciRP.org/journal/jbise/
JBiSE
).
Published Online December 2010 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/JBiSE
Modeling perisaccadic time perception
Andrea Guazzini1, Pietro Liò2, Andrea Passarella1, Marco Conti1
1Institute of Informatics and Telematics-IIT-CNR, Pisa, Italy;
2Computer Laboratory, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Email: andrea.guazzini@gmail.com
Received 29 July 2010; revised 15 August 2010; accepted 18 August 2010.
ABSTRACT
There is an impressive scarcity of quantitative models
of the clock patterns in the brain. We propose a me-
soscopic approach, i.e. neither a description at single
neuron level, nor at systemic level/too coarse granu-
larity, of the time perception at the time of the sac-
cade. This model uses functional pathway knowledge
and is inspired by, and integrates, recent findings in
both psychophysics and neurophysiology. Perceived
time delays in the perisaccadic window are shown
numerically consistent with recent experimental mea-
sures. Our model provides explanation for several
experimental outcomes on saccades, estimates popu-
lation variance of the error in time perception and
represent a meaningful example for bridging psy-
chophysics and neurophysiology. Finally we found
that the insights into information processing during
saccadic events lead to considerations on engineering
exploitation of the underlying phenomena.
Keywords: Brain Clock; Saccades; Time Compression;
Time Perception; Craniotopiccoordinates
1. INTRODUCTION
One of the most important research areas in neurosci-
ence focuses on the neural mechanisms of time percep-
tion and in particular on the link between visual percep-
tion and the brain clock(s); for sake of simplicity, we
will call this visual timing. In the last decade important
evidences of a strong distortion of the time perception
during saccadic eye movements have been attracted the
most the attention of researchers. Saccades represents
probably the perfect process to study ”visual timing”
both for the changing of external space mapping onto
retina and for the brain pathways alterations caused by
the saccadic neural correlates [1-6]. The numerical
simulation of the known saccadic effects by a model
which integrates the insights coming from psychophys-
ics and neurophysiology appear today to be a promising
scenario to extend the discussion on the brain clock(s)
problem. One interesting question is whether temporal
judgments is distributed or centralized [7]. The time
perception in the range of milliseconds to minutes in-
volves variety of neural population located in the frontal
cortex, hippocampus, basal ganglia and cerebellum.
Nowadays it is well known that the “internal” represen-
tation of time depends on the integration of multiple
neural systems. While there is good evidence about dif-
ferent clocks for different interval lengths, some recent
evidences point clearly to the existence of visually based
timing mechanisms. Researchers from psychophysics
and neurophysiology have approached this topic from
different sides, both leading to experimental evidences
that can be now merged together to obtain a more com-
plete perspective for visual timing processes than in the
past. The description of the neurological structures can
be performed at different scales. At the lower level of
description, the micro-scale is represented by single
neurons as nodes. Using a complex network framework,
we can assign certain properties of individual nodes (de-
gree, centrality, clustering, etc.); in order to model the
dynamical process each node is performing in process-
ing the signal. This level of description is unfortunately
computationally very expensive nowadays and very pre-
cise only for small networks; therefore it does not allow
a generic analysis of the global properties of the system.
At the other extreme, we have the higher level of de-
scription, the macro-scale, represented by the psycho-
physics where statistical properties of the network as a
whole. In the middle of these descriptions still remains a
huge space for different scales of descriptions that are
termed mesoscales, or intermediate scales. These scales
are understood as substructures (eventually sub graphs)
that have topological entity compared to the whole net-
work, e.g. populations/communities of neurons which
are functionally and/or structurally homogeneous. In
particular, the community detection problem concerning
the determination of mesoscopic structures that have
functional, relational or even social entity is still contro-
A. Guazzini et al. / J. Biomedical Science and Engineering 3 (2010) 1133-1142
1134
versial, starting from the a priori definition of what a
community is. The correct determination of the meso-
scale in complex networks is a major challenge. On the
mesoscopic level, researchers have examined neural
activity as measured by means of local field potentials or
dense intracranial electrode arrays in human and
non-human subjects. These approaches have stimulated
models of neuronal communication between nearby re-
gions. By contrast, macroscopic parameters of neural
activity have been used to assess the characteristics of
large neuronal assemblies that fire in synchrony and thus
generate detectable voltage or magnetic gradients on the
surface of the scalp. This paper originates from our par-
ticipation in the three years EU project MEMORY in
which large amount of data has been collected. On the
light of the MEMORY project and discussions with ex-
perimentalist we aim at devicing the model that could
answer the question of time perception during the sac-
cadic events.
1.1. Combining Neurophysiological and
Psychophysical Evidencies
The psychophysical studies of the relationship between a
visual stimulus (say a dot moving etc) and the perception
of the elapsed time between the start and end of the sti-
mulus (which is subjective) have produced important
results. The finding that perceived duration of a dynamic
stimulus can be manipulated in a local region of visual
space has confirmed that the visual perception of time is
processed directly by superior visual areas at least in its
early stages [8]. An interesting experiment has recently
shown that the existence of a large contrast effects in the
discrimination of short temporal intervals during fixation
events [9]. Della Rocca et al. examined the effect of
temporal distractors on interval discrimination and
showed that the perception of the time driven by visual
events is affected by early past experiences, as it hap-
pens for others characteristics related to the visual stim-
uli such as the shape, color or movement of objects. An
important finding comes from the studies of Morrone
and Burr about time perception during saccades [4].
Their major results has been that perceived time is com-
pressed when stimuli are ashed shortly before or after
the onset of a saccadic eye movement [1,2,10]. More-
over time and space perception seem to follow the same
compression pattern during saccades both for timescale
and magnitude of the effects. This evidence suggests that
time information is embedded with other characteristics
in the same neural representation of stimuli. The neuro-
physiological approach to the visual timing has revealed
neural correlates of visual experience which could ac-
count for the psychophysical findings [11-13]. The
hypothesis of an explicit neural representation of tem-
poral attributes of visual stimuli has a wide acceptance
[14]. From this point of view the fundamental represen-
tation of visual information should be able to encode
temporal information similarly to the other characteris-
tics of the perceived matter (color, frequency, bright-
ness). Previous studies have drawn the hypothesis that
cerebral circuits are inherently able to rescale durations
in a proportional manner and compensate for the error
differences generated by the cerebellum. Following such
assumption, to explain visual timing failures during sac-
cades an explicit model has to consider at least those
neural areas and pathways by which visual experience is
represented, filtered and analyzed [15]. In a recent paper,
Buonomano proposed that short-term plasticity and dy-
namic changes in the balance of excitatory-inhibitory
interactions may underlie the decoding of temporal in-
formation, that is, the generation of temporally selective
neurons [16]. He first showed that it is possible to tune
cells to respond selectively to different intervals by
changing the synaptic weights of different synapses in
parallel. Short-term plasticity is a usage-dependent
change in synaptic strength on the time scale of milli-
second to seconds and is observed in almost every syn-
apse types of the central nervous system. Each type of
synapse has its own specificity with respect to this prop-
erty. When stimulated a few times within a second, some
synapses show facilitation, others depression or else
complex sequences of facilitatory and depressing changes
[16]. According with the previous claim, recent results
show that for visual perception the timing attributes of
visual experience and the temporal tuning properties of
certain visual neurons are linked. Moreover neurons in
visual areas of primate parietal cortex have reduced la-
tencies to visual stimulation at the time of a saccade [3].
Several hypotheses of time measuring systems, peculiar
to neural circuits, demonstrates synchronization enables
both the evaluation of different time scales and the
binding between different sensorial/cognitive modalities.
Lesion and functional imaging studies have identified
frontal, temporal, and parietal areas as playing a major
role in the control of visual timing processing, but very
little is known about these areas interact to form a dy-
namic attentional network. We know that the fronto-
parieto-temporal (FPT) network communicates by means
of neural phase synchronization. Results reveal that
communication within FPT proceeds via transient long-
range phase synchronization in the beta bend [17]. Re-
cent results concern the high-frequency long range cou-
pling between prefrontal and visual cortex. Desimone et al.
used paired recordings in the frontal eye field (FEF) and
area V4, they found that attention to a stimulus in their
joint receptive fields leads to enhanced oscillatory cou-
Copyright © 2010 SciRes. JBiSE
A. Guazzini et al. / J. Biomedical Science and Engineering 3 (2010) 1133-1142 1135
pling between the two areas, particularly at gamma fre-
quencies [18]. This coupling appeared to be initiated by
FEF and was time-shifted by about 8 to 10 ms across a
range of frequencies. Considering the expected conduc-
tion and synaptic delays between the areas, this
time-shifted coupling at gamma frequencies may affect
the postsynaptic impact of spikes from one area upon the
other, consequently affecting visual timing [18]. This
long-range synchronization mechanism could be crucial
especially during saccades, where we know those 100
ms before the saccade onset the FEF releases a corollary
discharge signal towards the lateral intraparietal cortex
LIP, where a craniotopical representation of the envi-
ronment is implemented. The receptive fields of visual
neurons are known to be retinotopically arranged, and in
awake animals they move with gaze, maintaining the
same retinotopic location regardless of eye position.
Nevertheless the existence in the monkey parietal cortex
of cells (called “real-position” cells) whose receptive
field does not systematically move with gaze has been
proven [19]. These cells respond to the visual stimula-
tion of the same spatial location regardless of eye posi-
tion and therefore directly encode visual space in crani-
otopic instead of retinotopic coordinates [20]. The de-
synchronization due to the long-range coupling between
FEF and V4 tell us that the remapping process act ini-
tially on the older visual signal, while the present one
will be encoded later. Finally at least two considerations
have to be taken into account in order to elucidate the
neurophysiological implications on the visual timing
during saccades. As shown in Figure 1, the former is a
delay between the stimulus administration and its en-
coding into the craniotopical representation of the world
allocated in LIP, and the latter is that this delay is in-
creased at the time of saccades because of the different
neural networks involved in the fixation and the saccadic
state.
1.2. Communication Network Approach
In modern neuroscience, computational methods occupy
an important niche. Neuroscience computing is con-
cerned with building mathematical and statistical models
to address a wide range of questions originated from
experimental neurobiology and related medical fields.
Typically, these models involve massive, hard-
ware-demanding amounts of calculations performed on
last-generation computers. However, the complexity of
real-life biological systems is indeed overwhelming, far
outreaching the abilities of modern computing systems.
This creates a considerable demand for parsimonious
models which capture main driving forces of biological
processes elucidating the mechanisms behind the ob-
served phenomena. The search for a model which is
(a)
(b)
Figure 1. Schematic representations of the neural cir-
cuits of early stage of vision process, respectively in-
volved in the fixation condition (a-simplified neural
network of vision during fixation) and during a sac-
cadic onset (b-neural network during saccades). From
a to g are indicated the links among the vertex which
form the pathway.
complex enough to be a reasonable approximation of the
biological reality and at the same time simple enough to
be manageable by available mathematical techniques is
some sort of an art. The task requires the researcher not
only to have a thorough understanding of the relevant
biology and mathematics but sometimes a great deal of
luck to have the predictions of the model to be consistent
with the natural phenomena. Computational neurosci-
ence focuses on two distinct levels of description: ‘sim-
ple models’ and ‘detailed models’. While detailed mod-
els focus on the measurement and description of com-
plex systems through using certain building blocks, their
interactions and dynamic properties, such as kinetic pa-
rameters, binding constants etc. Simple models aim at
capturing the behavior of the few most relevant compo-
nents of the system. This implies that such models are
also intrinsically more abstract than detailed models
their components and parameters often do not corre-
spond directly to well-defined physical quantities, such
as measured binding constants or chemical reaction rates.
Copyright © 2010 SciRes. JBiSE
A. Guazzini et al. / J. Biomedical Science and Engineering 3 (2010) 1133-1142
1136
Nonetheless, simple models can provide insights into the
behavior of a system and drive experimental and detailed
modeling efforts by suggesting further more detailed
experiments or models. Moreover such modeling frame-
work could allow easier predictive multi-scale descrip-
tions of the system under study than detailed models.
Current detailed models are based on two different ap-
proaches: a bottom-up approach which focuses on the
measurement and description of complex systems using
the building blocks their interactions and dynamic prop-
erties, a top-down modeling approach attempts to de-
velop integrative and predictive multi-scale models of
the system under study. Our goal is to develop a hybrid
strategy between the two frameworks, focusing on spe-
cific problems at particular scales using a simple model-
ing and then expand on other scales. A main target of
using such combination of modeling approaches is to get
deeper understanding of the system without suffering
from sudden growth of degrees of freedom. A coherent
approach to study visual timing should take into account
the neurophysiological knowledge about the fronto-pa-
rieto-temporal network, providing for first a simplified
model of its functional structures. Afterwards throughout
a reverse engineering process would be possible to vali-
date the model fitting the psychophysical data. Such
approach can provides insights about the way by which
the brain has optimized its “software” to measure the
time in the absence of clock, with respect to its own bi-
ological constraints. In order to represent the complexity
of the visual timing processing, it seems to be essential
to define at least three computational objects. The first
object is an appropriate structure of the neural represen-
tation for the visual information. From psychophysics
we know mainly that visual information preserves a cra-
niotopic structure from early to late stages of brain anal-
ysis. From these evidences the system seems to be char-
acterizable by coupling neural pathways, where the neu-
ral signal coming from visual ganglion cells propagates.
Moreover real neural structures are able to embed spatial
and temporal information about visual stimuli in the
same neural representation, and by a phase of synchro-
nization dynamics among FPT, are able to represent di-
rectly the time information. The second aspect that is
necessary to manage relies on the signal propagation
dynamics along the structures defined above. As shown
by recent experiments the dynamics of signal propaga-
tion could have a role in the visual timing, and to repro-
duce their peculiarity both the synchronization of the
neurons’ activity and the dynamics of synapses have to
be taken into account. An appropriate representation of
the synaptic learning parameters is to incorporate in the
model the insights of Buonomano about the relation be-
tween synapses short term dynamics and the capacity of
neural structure to represent the timing aspects of a per-
ceived stimulus. The last behavior which has to be cap-
tured is the phase synchronization dynamics which cha-
racterized the FPT network. To face this challenge a
phase dynamics of the neural signal is explicitly mod-
eled, and a correlation between this parameter and signal
propagation is shaped by the equations of the model.
Finally the main attempt of this paper will be to high-
light the interplay between the signal diffusion and the
information diffusion. The paper proceeds as follows, in
the Section 2 the computational model is presented and
their component is elucidated. Section 3 describes the
numerical simulations of the psychophysical experi-
ments taken into account, and in Section 4 the results are
presented. Finally in the Section 5 numerical results and
insights are discussed and next steps and conclusion are
outlined.
2. THE MODEL
2.1. The FEF-LIP-V4 Network
The cerebral network FEF-LIP-V4 is represented by a
cellular automata composed by interconnected elements
labeled as nodes, where the retinotopic input propagates
and that allocate the spatiotopic layer. The cellular au-
tomata can be seen as having one ’space dimension’
which are nodes describing clustered or homogeneous
populations (e.g. column), and one orthogonal ’time di-
mension’ which encode the duration of the stimuli and
the interval among them (Figure 2). Our aim is to inves-
tigate the visual timing related aspects without introduc-
ing particular topology. As a consequence we image to
have a neurons lattice where the distances among nodes
are related to the transfer of the stimulus information,
from one node ito the next one i + 1, and are assumed to
be equivalent. The purpose of this equivalence of time
transfer is to avoid explaining time space aberration
through the introduction of a bias or fictitious, ad hoc
mechanisms. Cerebral pathways connecting V4 with LIP
and FEF are approximated by parallel neural delay chains
Figure 2. Schema of two signals moving from left to right,
which are identified by their phases (arrow) and its intensity
(arrow’s length). See legend on the left.
Copyright © 2010 SciRes. JBiSE
A. Guazzini et al. / J. Biomedical Science and Engineering 3 (2010) 1133-1142
Copyright © 2010 SciRes.
1137
of nodes, where the signal propagates synchronizing
subsequent nodes’ phase within a column, and modify-
ing the weight of synapses among nodes. The cellular
automata can be seen as having one ’space dimension
which are nodes describing clustered or homogeneous
populations (e.g. column), and one orthogonal ’time di-
mension’ which encode the duration of the stimuli and
the interval among them.
Each node represents a homogeneous population of
neurons; nodes are spatially organized in parallel col-
umns to reproduce the pathway followed by the neural
signal coming from visual area V4. Neurons inside a
node are assumed to synchronize their activity whenever
a signal arrives, e.g. to fire simultaneously, and are
linked together by synapses with a long term dynamics
[21]. The node is assumed as the “atomic” component of
the system and its behavior is explicitly modeled by the
definition of an activation state Si,j
t within the interval (0,
1). Given the physiological constraints three different
contributions to the signal have to be taken into account.
Besides the contribution of the direct synapses it is
needed to consider the lateral connections (transcolum-
nar contribution) and the respective signals’ phases in
order to calculate the resulting signal which is summed
to the target node. The synaptic weights inside a column
are indicated as (Wdi,j
t) while the transcolumnar synaptic
weights are indicated as (Wli,j
t).Using the trigonometric
formulas one can write the contributions as follows:

1,12
2 2
1, 11, 11,1, 11, 1
tanh 1
2
ij
Tijij ijijij
Wd
IS WlSS Wl

  

 
(1)
2
11,11,1,1 ,11,1,1
2cos
ij ijijijijij
ISWl SWd

 
 (2)

2
21, 11, 11, 11, 11, 11, 1
2cos
ij ijijijijij
ISWl SWd

 

(3)

2
3,1 ,11,11,1,11,1
2cos
ijijiji jiji j
ISWdSWl

 

(4)
JBiSE
The incoming signals are consequently computed us-
ing the Eq.5 and the activation state of each node are
finally updated according with the Eq.6.
1
,12
ˆt
ij T
SIII

3
I
1
,
t1
(5)
them a value φij
t within (-π, +π). Therefore the incoming
signals have to be described as having their own magni-
tude and phase. To estimate the resulting phase we use
again the trigonometric formulas:
11,11,
tt t
ij ij
SS Wl

(7)
1
,,
ˆ
tt
ijij ij
SSS
 (6)
2,1,
tt t
ij ij
SSWd
1
1
(8)
In order to introduce the long range synchronization
effects within the V4-LIP-FEF network the phase of
each node is explicitly considered assigning to each of 31,11,
tt t
ij ij
SS Wl

(9)
 
11,12,131,1
1
,
11,12,131,1
sinsin sin
ˆarctan coscos cos
tttt tt
ijij ij
t
ij tttt tt
ijij ij
SSS
SSS


 
 






(10)
Then the phase dynamics of a node has been coupled
with the magnitude of the signal coming from the con-
nected nodes and the state of the node itself, as follows:
 
 
,,,,
1
,
,,,,
ˆˆˆ
sin sin
arctan ˆˆˆ
cos cos
tttt
ij ijij ij
t
ij tttt
ij ijij ij
SS
SS


(11)
A way for the brain to encode in the same neural sig-
nal both time and space information about a stimulus,
could be to exploit the network V4-LIP as a sort of neu-
ral buffer. Assuming linked nodes which evolve ”rap-
idly” their connections weight according with the in-
coming signal features (e.g. activation and phase agree-
ment), it is possible to equip an artificial system with a
sort of embedded short memory of past signals or in
another words with a state dependent response behavior
to the signal. Such hypothesis has led Buonomano to
explore the role of synapses characterized by a short
term dynamics [16], demonstrating how a neural net-
work equipped with such synapses can represent both
spacial and temporal information about the stimulus by
the same neural correlates on a spatiotopic map. In order
to take into account the role of the synapses’ dynamics
on both the signal dynamics and the node’s synchroniza-
tion the equation for the synaptic enforcement has been
shaped including this features, according with the bio-
logical constraints. The synaptic weight (Wi,j
t) is defined
as a variable of the model belonging to the interval (0, 1).
A. Guazzini et al. / J. Biomedical Science and Engineering 3 (2010) 1133-1142
1138
To design the complex dynamics of the synapses two
ad-hoc parameters are designed and labeled respectively
learning factor (γ) and decay factor (δ), both defined
within (0, 1). Since the physiological characteristics of
the ocular dominance columns we introduced two dif-
ferent values to distinguish between the signals’ dynam-
ics inside and outside the column. Consequently we in-
dicate the intracolumnar and the transcolumnar learning
and decay factor respectively as γd and δd for the intra-
columnar synapses and γl and δl for the transcolumnar
(e.g lateral) synapses. Moreover two thresholds con-
stants are introduced to indicate the resting values for the
synaptic weight (Kw) and the nodes’ activity (Ks), both
within the interval (0, 1). The synaptic weight is finally
updated at each time step using the following recipe. If
the activity of the precedent node is greater than the ac-
tivity resting threshold (Ks), according to Hebb’s theory
[22], the synaptic weight increases with the signal mag-
nitude and the phase synchronization between the two
connected nodes. Otherwise the synapses would decay
proportionally with the distance between phases of con-
nected nodes and as a function of their activity (Eqs.13,
14).
,,1
1
,:, 1
min tt
ij ij
t
ijij

 (12)


2
11
,, ,:,1,
1
,:, 1,,
1
1
ttt t
ijij dijijijs
tt t
dijijij wij w
WdWdS K
Wd KWd K



 
  
(13)


2
11
,, ,:1,1,
1
,: 1,1,,
1
1
ttt t
ijijliji jijs
tt t
lijijij wij w
WlWlS K
Wl KWl K





 
 (14)
Finally the main assumption relies on the different
learning and decay functions for intracolumnar and ex-
tracolumnar synapses dynamics. In the Eqs.13 and 14
the parameters γd and γl rule the learning rate respec-
tively for intracolumnar and transcolumnar synapses.
2.2. Numerical Simulations
In order to study the phenomenology of the computa-
tional model we started grounding on few basic assump-
tions the design of the control parameters under consid-
eration. The decay dynamics is controlled by the pa-
rameters δd and δl. The different nature of intracolumnar
and transcolumnar connections is expressed also in terms
of signal propagation effciency. While the slow dynam-
ics of the transcolumnar synapses is coupled with a lin-
ear function of signal magnitude transmission [1,20,23],
the faster dynamics of the intracolumnar synapses has
been shaped by a sigmoidal behavior [24,25]. Another
fundamental assumption relies on the spatiotemporal
transformation at the neural level, here proposed as the
fundamental key-mechanism for the visual time percep-
tion. Our approach is based on a fundamental symmetry
between the neural buffer and the phases based theories
for the neural activities. Whether we assume the neural
propagation of the signal as a phase dynamics of differ-
ent neuronal populations the equations get biological
sense. Consequently the more computationally light ap-
proach has been adopted, implementing the time related
phase dynamics of a node as the degree of diffusion of
the signal along a neural delay chain. The retinotopic
input of a real stimulus is simulated by the imposition of
a pick of activation in those nodes of the first layer of the
automata, corresponding to the spatiotopic representa-
tion of the stimulus. The activation of the local popula-
tions of nodes is maintained for the entire duration of the
encoded stimulus. The nodes which encode the stimulus
are assumed as synchronized and characterized by the
same phase for the entire duration of the stimulus. At the
end of the stimulus the synchronization inside and within
the nodes decreases following the Eq.11. Among the
peculiarities of the saccades, there is the recalibration of
the spatiotopicityin the encoding stage of visual proc-
essing (LIP) which anticipate them. This recalibration is
reached by the FEF-LIP-V4 network by mean of an in-
ternal signal representing the eye position defined cor-
ollary discharge [26]. The corollary discharge (CD)
starts 60 ms before the saccade from the FEF and is
complete at saccadic offset producing an apparent en-
largement of the visual receptive fields, and a fast re-
mapping of the visual environment, biasing both time
and space detection ability. In order to simulate the CD
effects on the craniotopical representation of the visual
stimuli, in the remapping phase simply we shift the sig-
nal parallel to the direction of the saccade. In this way
while a stimulus temporally distant from a saccade (i.e.
long after or before) produces the same response in the
craniotopic map, during or in the temporal proximity
(50-100 ms) at the saccadic event the activation and
synchronization pattern of the map appear as altered
with respect to the fixation condition. As a consequence
of this alteration in the read out stage the system could
fail to estimate the time features of the stimuli. In hu-
mans the ”readout” task has to operate on the cranio-
topic representation of the early visual history, in order
to reveal both topological distances between stimuli and
timing features as duration of an interval between two
visual events. The model we have defined operates a
temporal to spatial transformation of the neural signal,
producing a representation of the craniotopic map allo-
cated in (LIP). The state of this map has been analyzed
by a two step procedure: for first the state of synchroni-
zation of the map were detected by a kohonen neural
Copyright © 2010 SciRes. JBiSE
A. Guazzini et al. / J. Biomedical Science and Engineering 3 (2010) 1133-1142 1139
network [27], obtaining the absolute “network position”
of their trace of synchronization and finally these posi-
tions were used by trained output neurons to estimate the
temporal localization of the stimuli. The system appears
to be able to detect veridically time and space localiza-
tion of a stimulus in fixation conditions Figure 3.
During the saccadic event two main consequences
could be postulated given our mesoscopicalmodelization
of the system. As shown in a previous work [28] the
former is determined by the remapping of the signal lit-
erally over previous activated pathway, at least in the
LIP neural district. The latter is the delay between the
peripheral detection of the signal and its encoding in the
(a)
(b)
Figure 3. Figure a and b represent the magnitude of activation
and the phases synchronization of the ensemble of nodes re-
spectively. The craniotopical map is idealized by a region de-
scribed in xycoordinates; in a we show the magnitude of nodes’
activation, and in b the phase difference with the first neigh-
bors. The color bars on the right indicate a normalized intensity
variation both for the activation and the phases differences.
LIP district due to the different pathways which are ac-
tivated. To fit the model parameters a real psychophysi-
cal experiment about time compression during saccades
has been taken into account and used as reference data-
set. In the considered experiment Burr et al. studied the
perception of time during saccades asking the subject to
compare the time interval between two couples of con-
secutive visual stimuli in two different conditions, dur-
ing fixation and during a saccadic event [2]. The ex-
perimental results have allowed us to analyze the rela-
tion between the temporal proximity of a saccadic event
and the magnitude of time compression. To simulate the
experimental paradigm we define two temporal variables
labeled respectively as Tsac for the interval between the
first stimulus administration and the saccadic onset, and
δTst to indicate the interval between the stimuli. The
final stage of the numerical simulations has generalized
the psychophysical paradigm described in Ross et al. [5]
about psychophysical effects of time compression during
saccades for different values of both the delay between
the visual stimuli and their temporal distance from the
saccade onset.
3. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
Following the schema described in Figure 2 we simulate
in Figure 3 the activation of nodes in a region of crani-
otopical map. Signals of different magnitude (a) are
identified by the change of phase (b). The first task of
the numerical simulations has been to validate the model
using the experimental data gently provided from Mor-
rone et al. [4]. After some preliminary simulations we
found how the ”transcolumnar” learning and decay fac-
tors only determine the lateral diffusion/synchronization
of the signal, having no direct effects on the time estima-
tion. As a consequence these parameters have been
treated as nuisance variables and set to values of con-
venience. Finally the “intracolumnar” learning and de-
cay factors have been estimated fitting the real data by a
standard least square method. First we defined a refer-
ence dataset starting from the real data, which provided
the time compression of two visual stimuli separated by
100 ms depending on the saccade’s onset time. Then we
compute the least square distances between the model
estimations and reference dataset for different values of
γd and δd (Figure 4). The best fitting curve is shown in
figure 6 and it is obtained for γd = 0.07 and δd = 0.08 and
characterized by an r2 = 0.93. The space of parameters in
Figure 4 shows those values for which the system is
characterized by a coherent behavior with the real data.
Finally these values have been assumed as parameters of
the model for the second phase of numerical simulations.
The second task of numerical simulation has been the
exploitation of the model in order to explore its behavior
Copyright © 2010 SciRes. JBiSE
A. Guazzini et al. / J. Biomedical Science and Engineering 3 (2010) 1133-1142
1140
Figure 4. Space of parameters for γd and δd respectively on the
X and Y axes. The color (see legend) indicates the normalized
least square distance between the model estimation and the real
data. The best fitting value is obtained for γd = 0.07 and δd =
0.08.
for different values of delay between the visual stimuli.
The estimation of the delay between two subsequent
stimuli separated by an interval of ΔTst is considered
both for different values of the saccadic onset Tsac, and
for different values of ΔTst. The percentage of time
compression is computed comparing the model’s interval
estimations in the fixation condition with those near the
saccade, and it was assumed as order parameter.
The normalized time compression among the two
conditions is reported using colors, with red for large
values of compression and blue for compression near to
0. The y axis indicates the delay between the time of the
saccade onset and the time of the first administered sti-
mulus while the x axis indicates the delay between the
couple of stimuli. Since the available experimental data
about this paradigm are only estimated for a delay
among the stimuli of 100 ms these results provide some
predictions/forecasting which would be interesting to
verify.
The Figure 5 shows how the maximum of compres-
sion is obtained always around the saccadic onset. Pairs
of stimuli separated by a little delay appear as more
compressed when administered largely before the sac-
cadic onset, and exists a critical values of 200 ms for
which no significant compression is revealed.
4. CONCLUSION
We have developed a model that provides a fit of experi-
mental data concerning time compression during saccades.
With this work we suggest a way to couple the cranio-
topical representation of space-time attributes of a visual
stimulus with the neurophysiological structure of the brain.
The model has been tested through using two parame-
Figure 5. Time compression (see legend) is indicated as the
normalized difference between the time estimation in the fixa-
tion condition and in the saccadic condition. In y axis we show
the delay between saccade onset and the first administered
stimulus. In x axis the delay between the stimuli is represented.
Figure 6. A cross-section of Figure 5 is shown for ΔTs = 100
ms. The vertical dashed line indicates the time of the saccadic
onset and the values on the x axis indicates the time delay of
the second stimulus administration. In the Y axis the percent-
age of compression is reported. Red dots indicate the experi-
mental observations and the continuous line represents the
model estimation.
ters which ruled the synaptic dynamics. Finally a fun-
damental mechanism for the process under scrutiny has
been identified as the interplay between the phase dy-
namics which characterize the neurons’ activity and the
synaptic dynamics. This model highlights how psycho-
physical and neurophysiological recent findings can
drive interesting explanations for time and space percep-
tion at the saccadic time and for the visual perception in
general. The simple implementation we propose for the
data analysis can be exploited in two main directions.
The former is the prediction of “new” and “unknown”
Copyright © 2010 SciRes. JBiSE
A. Guazzini et al. / J. Biomedical Science and Engineering 3 (2010) 1133-1142 1141
visual effects during saccade, by the numerical simula-
tion of arbitrary extreme setting conditions. Subsequent
experimental inspections would drive to important re-
finements for the model and to new theoretical questions
about the neural alghoritms used by the brain. The latter
is represented by a clinical exploitation of the model,
considering the evidences of the saccadic relate visual
behaviors in neuropathological conditions as a reference
dataset to be fitted [23,29]. We stress here that the mod-
eling of time perception at perisaccadic events is a
thriving field of research. It has two immediate and im-
portant benefits: the improved understanding of the
processes that shape saccadic events and our improved
ability to combine psychophysics and neuroscience to
address research on clocks in the brain. Multiple Sclero-
sis, Hungtington’s disease and Parkinson illness are only
three of the multitude of clinical pathologies which af-
fect both the neural phase dynamics and the synapses’
behavior [30,31,32]. Describing the different pathologies
in terms of their peculiar set of pathological effects al-
lows us to easily map them into the model. This last cru-
cial step would make of it a useful tool both to investi-
gate the expected time and space visual perception im-
pairment in those contexts and to explore new neu-
ral-related hypothesis for their explanation.
5. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This work is financed by the EU 6 Framework Programme Project:
Measuring and Modeling Relativistic-Like Effects in Brain and NCSs’.
We thank David Burr, ConcettaMorrone, Marco Cicchini and Paola
Binda of Pisa Vision Lab for suggestions.
REFERENCES
[1] Binda, P., Cicchini, G.M., Burr, D.C., and Morrone, M.C.
(2009) Spatiotemporal distortions of visual perception at
the time of saccades. Journal of Neuroscience, 29,
13147-13157.
[2] Burr, D. and Morrone, C. (2006) Perception: Transient
disruptions to neural space time. Current Biology, 16, 848.
[3] Ibbotson, M., Crowder, N. and Price, N. (2005) Neural
basis of time changes during saccades. Current Biology,
16, 834-836.
[4] Morrone, C., Ross, J. and Burr, D. (2005) Saccades cause
compression of time as well as space. Nature Neurosci-
ence, 8, 950-954.
[5] Ross, J., Morrone, C., Goldberg, E. and Burr, D. (2001)
Changes in visualperception at the time of saccades.
TRENDS in Neurosciences, 16, 472-479.
[6] Ross, J., Burr, D. and Morrone, C. (1996) Suppression of
the magnocellular pathway during saccades. Behavioural
Brain Research, 80, 1-8.
[7] Abeles, M. (1982) Local cortical circuits: An electro-
physiological study. Springer, Berlin.
[8] Johnston, A., Arnold, D.H. and Nishida, S. (2006) Spa-
tially localized distortions of event time. Current Biology,
16, 472-479.
[9] Rocca, E.D. and Burr, D.C. (2007) Large contrast effects
in the discrimination of short temporal intervals. Percep-
tion 36 ECVP.
[10] Burr, D. and Morrone, C. (2006) Time perception: Space
time in the brain. Current Biology, 16, 171-173.
[11] Lewis, P.A. and Walsh, V. (2005) Time perception: com-
ponents of the brain’s clock. Current Biology, 15,
389-391.
[12] Meck, W.H. (2005) Neuropsychology of timing and time
perception. Brain Cognition, 58, 1-8.
[13] Ullner, E., Vicente, R., Pipa, G. and Garca-Ojalvo, J.
(2008) Contour integration and synchronization in neu-
ronal networks of the visual cortex. ICANN, Lecture
Notes in Computer Science, 5164, 703-712.
[14] Orban, G.A., Van Essen, D. and Vanduffel, W. (2004)
Comparative mapping of higher visual areas in monkeys
and humans. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 315-324.
[15] Karmarkar, U. and Buonomano, D. (2007) Timing in the
absence of clocks: Encoding time in neural network
states. Neuron, 53, 427-438.
[16] Buonomano, D.V. (2000) Decoding temporal information:
A model based on short-term synaptic plasticity. The
Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 1129-1141.
[17] Gross, J., Schmitz. F., Schnitzler, I., Kessler, K., Shapiro,
K., Hommel, B. and Schnitzler, A. (2004) Modulation of
long-range neural synchrony reflects temporal limitations
of visual attention in humans. Proceedings of the Na-
tional Academy Sciences, 101, 13050-13055.
[18] Gregoriou, G.G., Gotts, S.J., Zhou, H. and Desimone, R.
(2009) High-frequency, long-range coupling between
prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science,
324, 1207-1210.
[19] Mark, J. and Morrow, M.D. (1996) Craniotopic defects
of smooth pursuit and saccadic eye movement. Neurol-
ogy, 46, 514-521.
[20] Galletti, C., Battaglini, P. and Fattori, P. (1993) Parietal
neurons encoding spatial locations in craniotopic coordi-
nates. Experimental Brain Research, 96, 221-229.
[21] Graham, B.P
. and Stricker, C. (2008) Short term plastic-
ity provides temporal filtering at chemical synapses.
ICANN, Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 5164,
268-276.
[22] Hebb, D.O. (1949) The organization of behavior. Wily,
New York.
[23] Btzel, K., Rottach, K. and Bttner, U. (1993) Normal and
pathological saccadic dysmetria. Brain, 116, 337-353.
[24] Abarbanel, H., Gibb, L., Huerta, R., and Rabinovich, M.
(2003) Biophysical model of synaptic plasticity dynam-
ics. Biological Cybernetics, 89, 214-226.
[25] Dittman, J.S., Kreitzer, A.C. and Regehr, W.G. (2007)
Interplay between facilitation, depression, and residual
calcium at three presynaptic terminals, Journal of Neu-
roscience, 20, 1374-1385.
[26] Ford, J.M., Brian, J.R. and Mathalon, D.H. (2010) As-
sessing corollary discharge in humans using noninvasive
neuron physiological methods. Nature Protocols, 5, 1160
-1168.
[27] Kohonen, T. (1982) Self-organized formation of topo-
logically correct feature maps. Biological Cybernetics,
43, 59-69.
[28] Guazzini, A., Lio, P., Conti, M. and Passarella, A. (2009)
Copyright © 2010 SciRes. JBiSE
A. Guazzini et al. / J. Biomedical Science and Engineering 3 (2010) 1133-1142
Copyright © 2010 SciRes.
1142
JBiSE
Information processing and timing mechanisms in vision.
ICANN, Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 1, 325-334.
[29] McGivern, R., Gibson, J., Jwnnings, D., Lavery, K. and
Montgomery,C. (2006) Normal and abnormal slowing of
saccades: are they one and the same phenomenon? An-
nals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Neurobiology
of eye movements: from molecules to behaviour, 956,
421-425.
[30] Huygena, P., Verhagenb, W., Hommesc, O. and Nico-
lasena, M. (1990) Short vestibule-ocular reflex time con-
stants associated with oculomotor pathology in multiple
sclerosis. Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 109, 25-33.
[31] Leigh, R. J., Parhad, I.M., Clark, A.W., Buettner-Ennever,
J.A. and Folstein, S.E. (1985) Brainstem findings in
Huntington’s disease: Possible mechanisms for slow ver-
tical saccades. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 71,
247-256.
[32] Mosimann, U.P., Müri, R.M., Burn, D.J., Felblinger, J.,
O’Brien, J.T. and McKeith, I.G. (2005) Saccadic eye
movement changes in Parkinson’s disease dementia and
dementia with Lewy bodies. Brain, 128, 1267-1276.