Psychology
2013. Vol.4, No.4, 420-426
Published Online April 2013 in SciRes (http://www.scirp.org/journal/psych) http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2013.44059
Copyright © 2013 SciRes.
420
Representations of the Future in Depression—A Qualitative Study
Ali Sarkohi1,2, Karin Forslund Frykedal1, Haze l Holmberg Forsyth2,
Staffan Larsson1, Gerha rd Anderss on1,3
1Department of Behavioural Sciences and Learni ng, Linköping University, L inköping, Sweden
2Department of Psychiatry , Linköping University Hospital, Linköping, Sweden
3Section of Psychiatry, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden
Email: ali.sarkohi@liu.se
Received January 14th, 2013 ; revised February 16th, 2013; accepted March 12th, 2013
Copyright © 2013 Ali Sarkohi et al. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons At-
tribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the
original work is properly cited.
Previous studies indicate that the ability to imagine negative or positive future events affects psychological
well-being and is a characteristic feature of depression. The aim of this study was to investigate how de-
pressed individuals v iew their future during different time periods. A total of 15 ind ividuals with a diagno-
sis of major depression were recruited from a psychiatry clinic and completed a semi-structured qualita-
tive interview. Questions were asked about the situation right now, before becoming depressed, and the
future (nearest time, within a year and the upcoming 5 - 10 years). Data were collected and analysed using
a qualitative approach inspired by grounded theory. The results showed that depressed individuals ex-
perienced a state of “ambivalence”, with negative cognitive, emotional, physical and socioeconomic con-
sequences when they were asked to think about the nearest future. Ambivalence and its negative emo-
tional and cognitive effects were substantially reduced in strength when they were asked about their more
distant future. We conclude that the concept of ambivalence in depression and/or anxiety in the present
may be an important feature of depression which deserves more attention from both a theoretical and
clinical perspective. The use of qualitative approaches in the study of depression is encouraged.
Keywords: Future Thinking; Depression; Ambivalence; Time Horizons
Introduction
It is known that the ability to imagine negative or positive fu-
ture events affect psychological well-being and can play an
important role in the process of recovery, persistence and re-
lapse of depression (MacLeod, Tata, Kentish, & Jacobsen,
1997). A characteristic feature of many depressed individuals is
a pessimistic and negative view of their personal future. This
clinical observation is covered in several conceptualisations of
depression (e.g., Beck, Rush, Sha w, & Emery, 1979; Abra mson,
Alloy, & Metalsky, 1989; Klinger, 1993). For example, Beck et
al. (1979) described a cognitive triad, where depressed indivi-
duals have a negative view of themselves, the world and the
future. Beck et al. argued that hopelessness about the future
play an important role in depression. Hopelessness, in turn, was
conceptualised by Abramson, Alloy and Metalsky (1989) as a
deficit of positive expectancy and excess of negative projected
thoughts. Macleod, Rose and Williams (1993) argued that the
despair about the future is characterized by the lack of positive
expectations, not necessarily by increased negative expectations.
According to Klinger (1993), hopelessness in depressed per-
sons often concerns future periods when the wished for goal
seems to be out of reach (e.g., “I will never get married”). Me-
lges and Bowlby (1969) argued that hopelessness is about re-
duced expectancy of success. In sum, several authors have ar-
gued the expectancies about the future is a central component in
depression.
In the early 1990s, MacLeod and his colleagues investigated
the significance of negative and positive expectations of the
future. They pointed out that research to date has focused on the
importance of negative thoughts about the future and devoted
less attention to the importance of positive expectations. They
further emphasized that positive and negative thinking has long
been regarded as opposite poles on a single scale. Subsequent
research has shown that positive and negative thinking rather
should be regarded as two separate systems, where increase in
one domain will not necessarily mean the decline in the other
(MacLeod & Moore, 2000). In order to measure future-oriented
thinking MacLeod et al. (1993) developed the Future Thinking
Task (FTT). The FTT is based on a verbal fluency task (Lezak,
1995), and the person is asked to generate as many positive and
negative anticipated events as possible within one minute along
various time periods, from the near future (within a week) to a
longer period of time (next year and the next 5 - 10 years). Re-
search on the FTT shows that parasuicidal individuals report
fewer anticipated positive experiences than controls, but no
overall increase anticipation of negative future experiences. The
results for depressed and non-depressed parasuicides are essen-
tially the same (MacLeod, Pankhania, Lee, & Mitchell, 1997;
Conaghan & Davidson, 2002). Previous studies with depressed
individuals show both an increase in expected negative events
and a reduction of positive events on the FTT (McLeod & By-
rne, 1996). In a recent study, we found that depressed persons
reported fewer positive future-oriented thoughts than the con-
trol group (Bjärehed, Sarkohi, & Anderson, 2010). These find-
ings indicate that a reduced anticipation of future positive
A. SARKOHI ET AL.
events is a characteristic feature of depressed individuals even
in the absence of suicidal ideation.
One limitation with the FTT is that the test may not capture
the qualitative aspects of how depressed individuals perceive
their future. First, the limited time given restricts the possibility
to reflect and situate the expected future within a broader
framework. Second, while the valence of the generated future
expected events is scored (e.g., likelihood) it may still not be
what the depressed person view as most important. Finally, as
the FTT rely on cognitive function (i.e., speed of processing),
some depressed may fail to generate future events on the FTT
while still having thoughts about the future. As far as we know
there is no qualitative study of depressed people’s thoughts
about the future in positive and negative sense, and along dif-
ferent time horizons, therefore, we adopted a qualitative ap-
proach. The aim of this study was to investigate the substantive
content of representations of the future in depressed individuals
by using open-ended methodology inspired by grounded theory.
A further aim was to gain knowledge that could inform future
investigations on the role of expectations about the future in
depression.
Methodology
Participants
A sample was selected who were under treatment at a psy-
chiatry clinic and volunteered to participate. A total of 15 in-
formants were included. They all had been diagnosed with ma-
jor depression using the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Asso-
ciation, 2000). There were 8 women and 7 men, and ages
ranged between 26 to 63 years. Participants differed in many
respects. For example many were on sick-leave and some were
unemployed. A few were inpatients due to earlier suicide at-
tempts. Different educational levels and occupational back-
grounds were represented, as well as differences regarding
marital status. For most onset of depression occurred after pu-
berty, but for a few the first episode debuted earlier.
Interview Guide
We prepared an interview guide inspired by Macleod et al.’s
(1993) FTT, but only used the questions from the FTT and
presented them in an open-ended version with no time con-
straints. The interviewer asked the participants what they thought
was going to happen in the future and added some other ques-
tions, including a question about the time before the partici-
pants became depresse d. More specifically, the interview guide
included questions about how participants look at their situation
right now, before they became depressed and how they saw
their future (nearest time, about a year and the coming 5 - 10
years). Moreover, in an open-ended manner we added questions
about how informants viewed their relatives’ future and their
thoughts about the future in Sweden, Europe and worldwide.
Finally, they answered questions about whether they saw any
obstacles and/or opportunities in their future. Any open ques-
tion was followed by one or more, follow-up questions to get
more detailed descriptions from the informants.
Procedure
Written and oral information about the study was provided to
informants by the psychiatry staff. In addition to information
about the purpose of the study etc., they provided data on re-
searchers’ names, phone numbers and email addresses in case
someone would like to ask questions about the study. Both the
written and oral data clearly revealed that participation was
voluntary and confidential, and that the contents of the inter-
view would not affect their on-going treatment at the clinic in
any way. Those who gave informed consent all met the criteria
of major depression using DSM-IV (American Psychiatric As-
sociation, 2000). Those with psychotic or manic symptoms, on-
going drug and/or alcohol abuse, current suicide plans and/or
had been depressed before but not at the studied time were not
asked to participate.
Included participants were contacted by a nurse to make an
appointment for an interview at psychiatric clinic, Linkoping’s
university hospital. The participants completed a semi-struc-
tured qualitative interview which lasted between 45 to 60 min-
utes. Interviews were recorded and transcribed after the inter-
view before the analysis. The interviews were conducted in a
quiet room in the clinic. The interviewers were trained in quail-
tative interview, aimed to be empathic, showing a genuine in-
terest in the participant, actively listened, and encouraged the
participant to explore their thought by using relevance follow-
up questions like: “would you tell me more about that?” The
study was part of an on-going clinic based study on the treat-
ment of depression for which ethical approval had been ob-
tained.
Data Analysis
The analysis started after the first interview and continued si-
multaneously . Grounde d theory (GT) method s ( Glaser & Strau ss,
1967; Glaser, 1978; Charmaz, 2006) were conducted to collect
and analyse data. The data collection process was guided by
theoretical sampling, where data collection and analysis ran in
parallel. By careful analysis, through substantive coding (open
and selective), we developed an interpretive understanding of
what the informants are occupied with and which was their
main concern. The main steps during the analysis of data were
coding (initial/open coding, focused/selective coding and theo-
retical coding), constant comparison, memo writing and memo
sorting.
The data were constantly compared with codes (e.g., don’t
dare, fear, worry, threat, focus on now, shame, guilt, bubble,
villain, anxiety, irresolute, apathetic, uncertain, pending, now-
past, now future, past, future, must, as-if, ruminate, suicide ide-
ation/suicide attempt etc.), subcategories (e.g., thought, feeling,
behaviour, oscillation, life line), categories (mood and time ho-
rizon), and core category (ambivalence). During the analysis we
wrote down our ideas about relationships between the codes
and other theoretical ideas that came to our mind, compared
and sorted them (memo writing and sorting).
Theoretical coding (e.g. consequences, causes, effects, strate-
gies, processes, etc.) was applied to understand how the sub-
stantive categories were related to each other, to the core cate-
gory and to derive a theoretical model. Theoretical codes are
used as tools to theorize the substantive codes (Stern, 2007).
The important issues in this process have been to examine if the
categories fit with the core category, subcategories, and how a
theoretical model can be built based on comparative analysis
between the data.
The open coding ended when the core category appeared.
Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 421
A. SARKOHI ET AL.
The theory was delimited to the core category. The core cate-
gory became a guide for further questioning (Glaser, 1978).
Follow-up questions in the last five interviews were theoreti-
cally sampled after the core category had emerged. The initial
codes were integrated to conceptualize and theorize how the
substantive codes and categories may relate to each other as a
hypothesis to be transformed into a theory (Glaser, 1978).
In the analysis we aimed to adhere to the Strauss and Corbin
(1998) principle of interaction between sensitivity (i.e. sensitive
proximity—the ability to feel/read the subtle nuances and hints
in the data) and objectivity (analytic distance—the distance be-
tween the empirical material and the emerging results). While
two of the authors have a cognitive-behavioural orientation we
tried, as much as possible, to keep an open mind (theoretical
agnostic) (Charmaz, 2006; Dey, 1999; Glaser, 1978).
The authors and an external person (colleague who had not
been involved in this study) compared the results and through
an interactive process of discussion, analysis and review of the
interview format, interview notes and the initial coding, arrived
at consensus on a refined coding. Using this coding, the re-
searchers independently coded the remaining interview notes
and met to compare different perspectives and interpretations of
ambiguous data and identification of concepts that were not
covered by coding. Differences could be reconciled without any
major disagreements. The researchers reviewed their notes and
coding to ensure that no significant concepts had been over-
looked, wrongly sorted or left uncoded. This resulted in coding
of any additional statements with existing encodings but no
new categories or disagreements with the encoding task.
Results
Participants’ representations of the future along different
time horizons (short and long term) are presented below, where
ambivalence has been distinguished as core category. It is im-
portant to note that the participants’ representation varied and
expressed in different ways. The results are based on the com-
mon pattern, but sometimes there are quotes that clarify varia-
tion.
Ambivalence
Recurrently and in various ways informants described a state
of ambivalence which was expressed in the following three
domains: thoughts, feelings and behaviour. Shifting between
choosing and not choosing, and to dare or not dare, took place
in the three time dimensions: the past and the present, the pre-
sent and the future with varying degrees of emotional, cognitive
and behavioural responses.
Partly, there was an oscillation between past and present,
where attempts to free themselves from the burden of the pre-
sent had failed (even suicide attempts) and created disappoint-
ment and depression. The disappointment seemed to permeate
the thoughts about the nearest future which lead to a generali-
zation of perceived disappointments over time. When infor-
mants did not make any significant difference between the past
and the possible future disappointments they became apathetic.
In such situations, they neither wanted nor dare to think and
believe in the future.
“Sometimes you feel like you have the future behind you.
I’m not as aware of this development as I would like to be,
because I have this “clinical picture”. ··· I think that I sit
sometimes and am quite lethargic, can’t get on with it.
And now the amount of Cipralex has increased from 15
mg to 40, which the body has responded with a couple of
side effects” (Male 57 years).
“I dare not think about the future because it never ends up
like I figured it” (Int.: Okay).
“I suppose that is something I’ve learned on the way. You
should stop thinking about the future because it is never
ends as expected” (Female 44 years).
For some there was an oscillation between the present and
the near future which gave an experience of “threat”, “fear” and
“worry”. The fear of facing new disappointments was consid-
ered as the “villain of the piece”, giving a sense of “going into a
bubble” and becoming increasingly depressed. This gave a feel-
ing of being “mentally handicapped”, which created suicidal
ideation and the thought that suicide was the only option. In ad-
dition to their depression and poor financial situation they were
afraid of losing their social network and possible employment.
“The worst is this panic anxiety. The depression is a bit
calmer. But that’s it, you go into a bubble. If you’d ask
me, you go very low, depressed or what should I
say? ··· It’s the thing with this panic anxiety and that.
That is what the culprit is. You can have a depression
even though you work. You sort of go in to your own lit-
tle world, but you still work. I wonder what percentage of
the Swedish population who suffer from depressions, but I
haven’t checked. It is probably fairly high, I think, but it’s
probably something “they can live with”. (Int.: But in
your case, there is the threat of getting sick again, as bad
as you were? ). Yes, I’m terrified··· and I daily think “what
will happen in 2 years?” I sit and worry about what will
happen in 2 years··· the authorities and people say “do
this, do that” and decide what I should do when I be-
come a different person. That is when I fear myself··· I
was very close to hang myself. Nice to just escape all this
stuff” (Male 42 years).
In order to avoid anxiety and experience of “being in a bub-
ble” the informants reported using various survival strategies
(e.g. “here and now focus”, “i s and should world”, “must-rules”,
“as-if-rate”, “suicide attempt” etc.). The strategies used varied
depending on the time dimension.
Ambivalence in Relation to Different Horizons of
Time
Future Thoughts about the Near Future
There was a desire to believe in oneself, one’s future, to form
an opinion, to make decisions and plan one’s near future, but
the informants reported that they “don’t dare”, since “fear of
not coping with my plans” raises “worry/anxiety of being dis-
appointed”. The near future wa s thus perceived as “threatening”.
Informants became immersed in negative thoughts and feelings
about what and how things would be done. Implicit and/or ex-
plicit wishes and decisions raised fear/worry. In addition to fear
and anxiety they felt “shame and guilt” for not having found
and/or provided long-term and constructive solutions to their
problems.
Ambivalence was evident and problematic when informants
confronted a problem and had to come up with a solution.
Copyright © 2013 SciRes.
422
A. SARKOHI ET AL.
Overall, making decisions regarding the near future was some-
times difficult. In such a situation, they experienced simultane-
ous contradictory positive and negative thoughts and emotions,
which made them behave increasingly uncertain, pending, ir-
resolute and sometimes apathetic.
The informants would like to have the positive things that are
associated with their choice and at the same time avoid the
predictable negative consequences. It seemed as if the positive
aspect of individual choice was overshadowed by the fear and
worry of the expected negative consequences. This may make
the individual switch between two positions, namely to dare to
choose or not choose their future plans. In the short term they
often described the fears and hopes in general terms.
“Many times I don’t dare. You become a bit··· I am also
like that in my personality that I··· I want that familiarity,
I want that sweet, nuclear family. eh.. oh.. I would like to
have thoughts of the future where things will be great and
to feel safe and to know whom you belong to, and so on.
But, many times it will not be as expected, as I imagined
and then I get so disappointed. (Int.: Okay). And it has··· I
divorced three and a half years ago and one of the main
causes for the divorce was that I was constantly disap-
pointed, from a mere trifle to bigger issues. (Int.: But if
we talk about the very near future. What are your thoughts
on that?). I don’t dare to think about the future since it
will never be as I imagined any way. (Int.: Okay). That’s
something I learned along the way. You have to stop hav-
ing thoughts of the future, it will never be as you expected
anyway” (Female 44 years).
Ambivalence and well-being affected each other. Ambiva-
lence could impair well-being which in turn exacerbated am-
bivalence. This created a vicious circle, making the depressed
individual more and more uncertain, pending, irresolute and
sometimes apathetic about making decisions about the near
future. The consequence may be that informants “go into a
bubble” or get stuck in a pre-existing bubble/depression.
“I can’t directly plan from day to day, for it depends on
exactly how I feel. ··· With depression and anxiety, you
kind of get it for free··· But with depression it has a lot to
do with thinking. You think and live in a small bubble,
and go in to one’s own world and think; now I’ll solve
this—maybe you can’t, but you’re still thinking it again
and again. So you go in to you little world and become
depressed. Yes, you don’t solve it and go deeper and
deeper down. (Int.: You do not plan to···). No, I don’t
dare. But if something might have happened. I’m not
saying, I might kill myself tomorrow, I don’t know. (Int.:
You feel that unstable?). Yes. I can’t directly plan from
day to day, for it depends on exactly how I feel. But the
worst is this panic anxiety. Depression is of course a bit
calmer. But that’s it, you go into a bubble. It is all about
getting very low, depressed or how to say it. (Int.: But do
you think it is how you feel that affects you that you are
not able to think in the near future?) Yes, sure” (Male 42
years).
Depression and its adverse consequences affected the indi-
vidual’s ability to function both in social life and at work. It
was considered as an obstacle for thinking about the near future.
Depression lead to feelings of “guilt, shame” and “fear of being
labelled” for life, and could lead the individual to “hide” the
problem in social contacts.
“I feel, mostly when I am sick, there is a lot of shame.
And even though there are many people who are de-
pressed around, there’s still a sense of shame anyway. To
some, I can’t say that I have a depression because it has a
negative tone. Oh, and I have heard many of those who
don’t know that I have depression, say “yes, they will of
course be labelled all their life” and it’s like that··· of
course it worries me. ··· Who would want to employ
someone who may be sick?” (Female 44 years).
Requirements and expectations from relatives, employers,
insurance service, employment services, social services etc. were
reported as affecting the informants negatively.
“The public employment service, social insurance, social
services and society require a lot from me. Then I get pa-
nic anxiety··· people say do this, do that, and decide what
I should do, then I became another person. Then I get fri-
ghtened of myself” (Male 42 years).
The situation created a dependency in relation to publicly
funded health care and health insurance, which participants
viewed as limiting the possibility of using their own resources
to cope with everyday life and to have the courage to plan ahead.
Sick leave and medication were described as necessary, but
informants also said they would have preferred not to have the
side effects of medication. They also disliked the prospect of a
continued poor economic situation, unemployment, fear of be-
ing stigmatized, and be dismissed from the labour market due
to mental illness.
Getting better was experienced as time-consuming and out of
reach in the near future. Therefore different life strategies were
used to avoid the uncertainty, fear and anxiety that were associ-
ated with decision making. In this mode, they reported they
would use a “here and now” focus, “must rules” and “displace
radical plans” further on in the future.
“I delimit it to cope with it, and it means I take one day at
a time. I don’t think, I get very stressed when I am think-
ing about the future ahead, because I’m still feeling bad. I
feel bad in myself. I’m pretty handicapped, I call it mental
paralysis. So I sort of think··· today I’m coming here at
two o’clock and then I’m going shopping. That’s what I
do today and then I have to look at what to do tomorrow,
if it is anything I must do. Then I’ll sit down tonight and
think about it. (Int.: Then you start thinking about the next
day?). Yes, basically if there is not anything you have to,
like I know that I booked an appointment with my phy-
siotherapist on Wednesday and it’s only because I must
cooperate, otherwise I’d rather decide at the same day.
(Int.: But you take one day at a time for that?). To be able
to handle it. Otherwise I start thinking about things that
are threatening, that I should start working again, and then
I panic because I am not ready for it” (Male 57 years).
The informants were aware about the impo rt ance of engaging
in positive activities. The problem was that anxiety and am-
bivalences made it difficult to dare doing anything at all and
lead to a feeling of living in two worlds namely “is and should
world”. Anxiety seemed to be “the villain of the piece” and
something informants did everything to avoid.
Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 423
A. SARKOHI ET AL.
Future Thoughts within a Year
Even if one year is still perceived as a “too short time per-
spective” some informants dared to think forward, however in
both positive and negative terms and in general and/or specific
terms. Once again, they expressed fears and worry to be disap-
pointed, but at the same time they expressed a cautious hope for
a turnaround, where they could do some things (start work/stu-
dy) and have good relationships.
“I have been ill for a long time and I have felt so terri-
bly bad. You struggle just to survive the day and if you
ask me “why do I do that?” if it still only gets worse. I
think I ought to have crossed the border of pain, surely
it can’t get any worse” (Male 43 years).
“It was hard but I hope it is better and that I have come
out of my depression and can live a normal good life.
But I don’t know, I hope so anyway. (Int.: What is a
normal life for you?). Go out without fear and not feel
stupid and being able to do stuff I can’t do today, go to
the store and things like that” (Female 26 years).
Despite some cautious hope informants expressed a fear of
“not embracing change” and/or to “make mistakes”. Further-
more, self-blame and self-devaluation occurred regardless of
the fact that they realized that the environment had signalled
other things than their own perceptions.
“I am, as they say sensitive to change so I think it’s really
hard to change; actually··· there is an element of uncertainty.”
(Female 35 years).
“I want to move to Norway. ··· Of course now, I need se-
curity and moving would be disastrous for me. Here, I have
some friends in town and moving to a new environment
would probably be dangerous for me. I would not be able to
handle it now. (Int.: But if you feel better do you think this
will come back to you, this future thinking and moving?). It
was like having some dreams that I did not implement.
Having some thoughts that one might··· But those dreams,
I never r eal ly live d the m, it was mor e a bout wha t yo u coul d
do with your future··· it might be too big a change for me.
(Int.: If you could live out those dreams? ). Ye s, I th ink so . I
would take on more than I could manage. It’s not just that,
for wherever I move, I’ll have no friends, no social envi-
ronment, nothing to anchor to in the first place and one just
has to work with that” (Male 43 years).
Future Thoughts within 5 - 10 Years
Informants were aware that depression had strong negative
effects on their views of the near future, but in the long time
perspective they hoped that depression would lead to some
positive opportunities. In spite of chronic depression and ex-
periences of previous unsuccessful treatments, there was a hope
of and a longing for a therapeutic effect in the long run. In the
long term they began both to wish and dare to think and plan
ahead. Hope was often expressed in general terms, but some-
times in more specific terms. They felt more energetic and
mentally on the way out of “the bubble”.
“Yes (laughs). Then there would be a positive scenario in
which I continue to be employed in any voluntary work
and who knows, I might stumble on something that means
a financial support in this. Or rather a contribution to the
pension you to the normal retirement pension” (Male 63
years).
“I might dream. I’ve done it before. (Int.: What are you
dreaming about?). I would like to move to Norway, partly
because you make more money and partly because I love
the culture there” (Male 43 years).
“Where I hope to be very happy, feel good, be like an or-
dinary person. And I would hope to have children, living
an average Swedish life” (Female 31 years).
In the long term, informants were more willing and recap-
tive to major changes in both family and working life. They
could for instance imagine that they would separate from
someone and/or move in with someone, have children, go
from sick leave to their former occupations (e.g., job/studies)
or apply for new jobs.
It was not uncommon to condition their future positive
thoughts by expressing themselves in “as-if” terms, which
can be considered as another lifeline/survival strategy in the
long perspective of time in the same way as “must rules’ in
the limited time frame. Views on depression related to plan-
ning their own future, to learn from their life history and to
help others. Informants occasionally told us that “there is a
deeper meaning with the condition” and you should not only
see the illness as a barrier but also to wonder about what po-
sitive things depression can bring.
“If I get well, I can help other depressed people, I can be
more empathic and have a greater understanding of their
condition··· I will not bring my feeling, but bring ex-
perience” (Male 46 years).
Finally, some in formant s made no distinct ion between th oughts
and feelings in a traditional way. Informants seemed to believe
that feelings are to be considered as a form of thoughts, causing
individuals with depression to feel that they are not able to
solve problems. In other words, the problem in depression to
some of them was the emotionally coloured thoughts which
were seen as maintaining the depression (instead of rational
thoughts).
“I would like to put it like this, there are two parts. There
are these emotional thoughts, or what to call them. And
then there are these more rational thoughts. But the ra-
tional thoughts are pushed away by the emotional, since
they are so much stronger no matter what. Purely rational,
I think that it doesn’t need to be like this, it will resolve
itself, and be this or that. But emotionally, I think that I
will not be able to manage to arrange it to be just this and
that” (Male 46 years).
Model of Past and Future Thoughts about the Future
In Figure 1 we describe a derived model of how the
depressed informant may oscillate between thoughts about the
future and thoughts about the past. In the near past the person
may predominately remember “bad” things, and in the near
future the person may also foresee aversive experiences and an-
xiety about the future. However, when thinking about the more
distant future and past the emotional valence may decrease.
These possible future experiences and recollections of good
events in the past are however not accessed as the person wan-
ders between the near past and the very near future. In therapy
Copyright © 2013 SciRes.
424
A. SARKOHI ET AL.
The past The future
N
ow
D
epression
A
nxiety
TIME
MOOD
Figure 1.
Model of ambivalence. The strength of depression and anxiety
varies as a consequence of the time perspective. Ambivalence is
most present (“the bubble” i.e., gray zone), when depression
and anxiety peak in the here and now.
it is common to focus on both past experiences and future plan-
ning and hence the person may be helped to overcome being
stuck oscillating between the near future and the near past. The
model shows that retrospective and prospective negative thought s
create and maintain anxiety and depression. Retrospective ne-
gative thoughts maintain depression and prospective negative
thoughts create and maintain both anxiety and depression. As a
way to avoid the discomfort associated with co nflicting thoughts
different survival strategies are applied depending on which
time dimension they are in with their thoughts. Retrospective
and prospective thoughts along different time dimensions may
raise different types of emotional reactions. On the one side
coexistence of both negative and positive prospective cogni-
tions on the other side coexistence of both negative retrospec-
tive and negative prospective cognitions in the present moment
without applying survival strategies seems to create ambiva-
lence. This in turn may increase the risk for suicidal behaviour.
Discussion
The results of this study indicate that depressed individuals
are experiencing a state of “ambivalence” with negative cogni-
tive, emotional, physical and soci o-economic consequences when
they are asked to think about the nearest future. Ambivalence
and its negative emotional and cognitive effects are substan-
tially reduced in strength when they were asked to think in a
longer time perspective such as 5 - 10 years.
Ambivalence creates a multifaceted picture where time is an
important factor. This means that ambivalence is significant in
the present moment in terms of thoughts, feelings and beha-
viours. Further into the future the ambivalence is reduced and
worry also decrease. Thoughts about the distant future can even
operate freely from ambivalence and anxiety. Ambivalence in
the moment was created by a fear of being disappointed and
once again fail. This ambivalence, caused by the fear of disap-
pointment, is a seemingly insurmountable obstacle of getting
out from the described “bubble”. Ambivalence can lead to a
feeling of hopelessness, which according to Beck et al. (1979)
has an obstructive effect and plays an important role in depres-
sion and we suggest that it plays an important roll in anxiety as
well. Hopelessness may be due to past memories, lack of posi-
tive future thoughts, or a surplus of negative future thoughts,
but it might as well be due to a few qualitatively strong and
persistent negative future thoughts. The ambivalent individual
behaves uncertain, pending, irresolute and sometimes ap athetic.
Several researchers have commented on ambivalence in de-
pression. For example, the concept of “cognitive dissonance”
(Festinger, 1957) can be applied, where one’s thoughts/ideas
are not consistent with each other, because the contradictory
positive and negative thoughts appear almost simultaneously.
This may consequently leads to a negative emotional and phy-
siological resonance (emotionally and physically response in
negative terms), with negative socio-economic consequences.
Ambivalence has also been found in interpersonal relations,
such as adult attachment (Busch, Rudden, & Shapiro, 2004).
The intensity and frequency of the contradictory thoughts
and individual internal/external resources to deal with these
thoughts may be critical to the strength of emotions experience
in depression. Oscillation between past and present, as found in
this study, may be a useful strategy in the short run but instead
of positive thoughts the depressed person may get stuck be-
tween anxiety and depression. In other words both thinking
back and about the future may be a negative experience as the
person fails to see beyond the very near present unless prom-
pted. As a way to avoid past unpleasant memories and/or un-
pleasant threats in the future the depressed person may apply
various survival strategies. The informants in this study re-
ported that they were willing and able to think positively about
their future, although in general terms, but that they were afraid
to experience new disappointments. It is understandable that
this situation often leads to avoidance. This may occur when an
individual chooses to refrain from approaching a seemingly de-
sirable situation in order to avoid experiencing any negative
consequences that may be associated with it. It is a condition in
which the individual perceives fear for something they wish.
When the target is far away, both positive and negative feelings
about the situation are less strong. Failing to regulate the cogni-
tive dissonance can create disharmony and experience in the form
of anxiety, guilt, shame, anger, insecurity, stress and other ne-
gative emotional states.
For the ambivalent depressed individuals the “here and now
focus” becomes a lifeline and a choice of situations that do not
require any immediate decisions. A disadvantage of this strat-
egy, like other life-lines set out in the “must-rules” and “if-as”
terms, is that the individual will not dare to think, plan and
make decisions about future goals. Here and now focus will be
of help only when the individual dare to set up concrete goals.
However, in clinical situations it is always up to the individual
to choose to expose themselves and strive towards their goals.
It is interesting to note that the concept of mindfulness has been
endorsed in the study of depression (Segal, Williams, & Teas-
dale, 2002), but we believe that the strategies used in mindful-
ness (e.g. being in the present moment) are not the same as the
“here and now” focus we saw in our informants which may be
more motivated by avoidance. In literature it is often mentioned
that depressed individuals have a pessimistic and negative view
of the future, but the results of this study show that this percep-
tion may mainly only concern the very near future and that the
distant future may not be viewed as negatively (e.g., Beck et al.,
1979; Abramson, Alloy, & Metalsky, 1989). This pessimism
was apparently rooted in both the depressive thought content
and the worry for the near future disappointments. Anxiety was
said to be the “villain of the piece” and the ambivalence curve
followed essentially the same curve as for depressive thoughts,
i.e. the closer in time the more anxiety. While we separate anx-
ious and depressive thoughts in this study, based on the reports
from our informants, the current evidence strongly suggest that
Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 425
A. SARKOHI ET AL.
Copyright © 2013 SciRes.
426
anxiety and depression overlap (Hirschfeld, 2001).
This study has limitations. First, the results were based on a
limited number of interviews with volunteer participants who
were recruited from an outpatient psychiatric clinic. While this
may be enough to capture the phenomena of future beliefs, it is
likely that other subgroups of patients, for example with less se-
vere depression, fewer comorbid problems, and dropouts from
treatment are not represented. A second limitation concern the
risk that our group of informants, because of their current de-
pressed state, were not able to provide full answers to the open
questions. If this is correct informants may have more full rep-
resentations of the future when feeling better. On the other hand,
this is less of a problem for our study as informants had long-
standing problems and not only a single episode of depression.
Third, as with most research, our observations and conclusions
are subject to potential bias from our preconceptions and preju-
dices. Finally, it is important to note that in this study, we have
chosen to highlight the ambivalence from a cognitive perspec-
tive and in the interview guide explicitly asked informants about
different time periods which might have not been discussed
with a less structured interview. On the other hand, our findings
are not markedly different from clinical reports on what de-
pressed persons say about their future (Beck et al., 1979).
We conclude that qualitative approaches can be useful to add
more knowledge on how depressed persons perceive their fu-
ture and our hope is that the concept of ambivalence in depres-
sion and/or anxiety in the present gets more attention from both
a theoretical and clinical perspective.
Acknowledgements
This study was sponsored in part from a grant by the Swed-
ish Science foundation to the last author.
REFERENCES
Abrahamson, L. Y., Alloy, L. B., & Metalsky, G. I. (1989). Hopeless-
ness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychologi-
cal Review, 96, 358-372. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.96.2.358
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical ma-
nual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington DC: American Psy-
chiatric Press.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive
therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press.
Bjärehed, J., Sarkohi, A., & Andersson, G. (2010). Less positive or
more negative? Future directed thinking in mild to moderate depres-
sion. Cognitive Behavio u r T h e r apy, 39, 37-45.
doi:10.1080/16506070902966926
Busch, F., Rudden, M., & Shapiro, D. (2004). Psychodynamic treat-
ment of depression. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Publish-
ing, Inc.
Charmaz, K. (2006). Constructing grounded theory. London: Safe Pub-
lications.
Conaghan, S., & Davidson, K. M. (2002). Hopelessness and the antici-
pation of positive and negative future experiences in older parasui-
cidal adults. Brit ish Journal of Clinic al Psychology, 41, 233-242.
doi:10.1348/014466502760379208
Dey, I. (1999). Grounding grounded theory: Guidelines for qualitative
inquiry. San Diego: Academic Pre ss .
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL:
Row, Peterson.
Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory:
Strategies for qualitative research. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Glaser, B. G. (1978). Theoretical sensitivity. Mill Valley: The Sociol-
ogy Press.
Hirschfeld, R. M. (2001). The comorbidity of major depression and
anxiety disorders: Recognition and management in primary care. Pri-
mary Care Companion to the Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 3, 244-
254. doi:10.4088/PCC.v03n0609
Klinger, E. (1993). Loss of interest. In C. G. Costello (Ed.), Symptoms
of depression (pp. 43-62). New York: Wiley.
Lezak, M. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York:
Oxford University Press.
MacLeod, A., Rose, G. S., & Williams, J. M. G. (1993). Components of
hopelessness about the future in parasuicide. Cognitive Therapy and
Research, 17, 441-455. doi:10.1007/BF01173056
MacLeod, A. K., & Byrne, A. (1996). Anxiety, depression and the an-
ticipation of future positive and negative experiences. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 105, 286-289.
doi:10.1037/0021-843X.105.2.286
MacLeod, A., Pankhania, B., Lee, M., & Mitchell, D. (1997). Parasui-
cide, depression and the anticipation of positive and negative future
experiences. Psychological Medicine, 27, 973-977.
doi:10.1017/S003329179600459X
MacLeod, A. K., Tata, P., Kentish, J., & Jacobsen, H. (1997). Retro-
spective and prospective cognitions in anxiety and depression. Cog-
nition and Emotion, 11, 467-479. doi:10.1080/026999397379881
MacLeod, C., & Moore, R. (2000). Positive thinking revisited: Positive
cognitions, well-being and mental health. Clinical Psychology and
Psychotherapy, 7, 1-10.
doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0879(200002)7:1<1::AID-CPP228>3.0.CO;
2-S
Melges, F. T., & Bowlby, J. (1969). Type of hopelessness in psycho-
pathological process. Archives of General Psychiatry, 20, 690-699.
doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1969.01740180074007
Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindful-
ness-based cognitive t h e r a py f o r d e p re s s i o n. New York: Routledge.
Stern, S. L. (2007). On solid ground: Essential properties for growing
grounding theory. In A. Bryant, & K. Charmaz (Eds.), The SAGE
handbook of grounded theory (pp. 114-126). Los Angeles, CA: Sage
Publication.
Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of qualitative research (2nd
ed.). Thousand Oak: Sage P ub l i c a t i o n s.