Advances in Physical Education
2012. Vol.2, No.4, 153-162
Published Online November 2012 in SciRes (http://www.SciRP.org/journal/ape) http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/ape.2012.24027
Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 153
Children’s Participation in the Design of Physical Activities
Conducted in the Outdoors
Fiona Wood1, Fraser Bruce2, Seaton Baxter3
1Institute of Health and Welfare, Robert Gordon University, Aberdeen, UK
2Duncan of Jordanstone College of Art and Design, University of Dundee, Dundee, UK
2The Centre for the Study of Natural Design, University of Dundee, Dundee, UK
Email: f.k.wood@dundee.ac.uk
Received September 17th, 2012; revised October 20th, 2012; accepted November 4th, 2012
Background: Despite recent UK Governmental policy changes concerning the amount of outdoor, physi-
cal activity children are currently engaged in (LTS, 2010), there is a gap in the academic literature con-
cerning children’s personal preferences for structured play and learning through physical activity in out-
door environments. Aim and objectives: This research explores the context of residential outdoor learn-
ing; with a particular focus on the contribution this experience may have on children’s preferences for
learning and play through healthy physical activity in the outdoors. Methods: Eco-analysis and Personal
Construct Psychology experiments (Catherine Ward-Thompson, 1995) were conducted with twenty chil-
dren (aged eleven to twelve years), participating in a five-day residential stay at an outdoor learning cen-
ter in order to ascertain their play preferences before and after their stay. Findings: Analysis of the data
indicated that children had a preference for outdoor, physical play activity. However, their usual play
typically involved only indoor, sedentary activity (e.g. games consoles and television) whilst at home.
Evidence suggests that the desired play of children is being ignored in favor of the “plugged in environ-
ments” (Louv, 2005) found in the modern family home. Conclusion: This research highlights the impor-
tance of implementing outdoor learning policies and practice into the current UK curriculum on a more
regular basis for the benefit of young children’s health and physical wellbeing. This article concludes with
future recommendations for the implementation of new strategies for outdoor learning providers that
would support and extend children’s physical activity in the outdoors without destroying their enjoyment,
exploration or play.
Keywords: Structuredplay; Nature; Children; Outdoor Learning; Physical Activity
Introduction
Our concern in this research and expressed in this paper is to
understand the context, from a child’s perspective, of learning
in the outdoors (in a residential setting) in order to develop
better participatory programmes which integrate healthy physi-
cal activities with ecological appreciation and learning.
More than ever before, young people are being severely and
sometimes unfairly criticised for their aberrant behaviour. This
can range from vandalism to random acts of violence and even
murder. Much of this is inexcusable but poorly understood.
Some of our best intellectual youngsters can succumb to the
pressures to compete exerted by family, friends and society as a
whole. In this “hothouse” environment (Quart, 2006) many
youngsters end up on drugs, prescribed or otherwise, in ma-
rauding gangs, or ultimately “go postal” (Ames, 2007), kill and
commit suicide.
Many children have been divorced from or deprived of the
very skills and experiences now needed to face up to and chal-
lenge the many crises in the world, for example, climate change,
loss of biodiversity, social inequity and so on. Without these
early skills and experiences, the problems are denied or cir-
cumvented with diversionary tactics and the world becomes a
worse place for their future. Many diversionary tactics find
their catalysts in the world of designed artefacts and powerful
advertising. From an ecological perspective, however, children
are becoming alienated from nature and suffer from what Louv
(2006) refers to as “nature-deficit disorder”, just at a time when
we need to be more connected in order to understand and cor-
rect the problems we ourselves have created in an unsustainable
world.
In 2004, the Scottish Government developed a new initiative
for Scottish education called “A Curriculum for Excellence”
(CfE) aimed at preparing children for adult life in the 21st
Century (SEED, 2004). This new educational agenda offered a
child-centered approach to encourage children to become: 1)
Successful learners; 2) Confident individuals; 3) Responsible
citizens; and 4) Effective contributors to society. A review by
the UK Minister of Education (2004), confirmed that outdoor
learning could play an important role in satisfying the deve-
lopment of children’s early learning of these four capacities,
whilst also contributing significantly to their involvement in the
natural world (LTS, 2007). These political concerns may be the
result in part, of recent evidence suggesting that children are
now not only less active, but they are less risk aware and de-
monstrate a decreased interest in the natural world, often fa-
vouring, instead, “plugged-in” play environments (Louv, 2005;
Gill, 2007, 2010; Honore, 2008).
So, it appears that learning in the outdoors can reconnect
children with nature and their local communities whilst provid-
ing them with the opportunity to develop the skills necessary to
F. WOOD ET AL.
recover from an ecologically impoverished society. To an even
greater extent, outdoor learning promotes physical activity in
all aspects of learning, leading to positive impacts on the par-
ticipant’s health and wellbeing (FPH, 2010). However, whilst
children are naturally active in their own play and games (i.e.
unstructured play activities), Physical Education (PE) and
adult-directed sports in primary and secondary schools do not
appear to increase the physical activity levels of children in the
same way (Gnanapragasam, 2010). However, participation in
sports can lead to improved physical fitness, self-esteem, social
capability and team working skills (Hughes, 1999). Nonetheless,
it would seem, that outdoor learning can make its contribution
to a recovery from these currently unacceptable outcomes and
help to restructure a new way forward, especially in the explo-
ration of children’s early learning and physical spontaneity.
Research Background
Children’s Learning through Play
Children’s play embraces the educational, recreational and
communicational rights of a child (West, 1996). It is a multidi-
mensional concept. It has long been recognized as an important
aspect of a child’s learning and development (Dansky, 1999),
allowing them to express their emotions and deal with everyday
situations (James et al., 1998). Petersen (1988) points out that
children who have had little opportunity to play can develop
serious behavioral problems later in life, such as, learning dif-
ficulties and phobias. From a developmental perspective, play
can be described as a freely chosen activity required for chil-
dren to grow into future successful adults (Holloway and Va-
lentine, 2000). Play is a key requirement to the healthy mental,
physical, and emotional development of children (Elkind, 2007).
Evidence suggests that children learn most effectively through
their self-directed play experiences (Power, 2000). So, can we
expect children through self-directed play to construct their
own practices of healthy physical education?
Children and Adult Directed Play
Play is well known as a behavior that does not need to be
watched, supervised, prearranged or encouraged by adults
(Schwartzman, 1983). This is child-directed play and is charac-
terized by activities created by the child that involve minimal
equipment and are conducted out with an adult’s supervision
(Adler & Adler, 1998).
Child directed play is “self-initiated” and is free from the ex-
ternally imposed rules or constraints common in adult-directed
activities (Dansky, 1999). From a developmental perspective,
child directed activities teach children a variety of essential life
skills: communication, resilience, self-reliance, compassion,
negotiation, resourcefulness and so on (Adler & Adler, 1998).
In addition, Asthana and Revill (2008) would also add deve-
loping friendships, risk awareness and confidence to this list.
A common feature of play in outdoor environments is the use
of games. Indeed, children often develop and engage in games
through choice as well as when instructed to do so. All over the
world, children play and create games as one of their most
popular physical activities (Piaget, 1962). It is through these
self-directed-games that children gain the opportunities for
important informal and social learning (Lever, 1978). There are
many reasons why children might engage in self-directed
games. For example, sometimes they are played for enjoyment,
pleasure and refuge or for social acceptance, whilst at other
times they are just for passing the time. Furthermore, child-
directed-games, or games-with-rules, provide a multitude of
cognitive skills necessary in later adult life (Hughes, 1999).
These may include, for example, the development of moral
values (Piaget, 1962), group or teamwork skills (Lever, 1978),
informal learning environments important to children’s deve-
lopment (Roussou, 2004), or strategic thinking through the
structuring of rules (Lever, 1978).
However, not all play leads to games because, according to
Gazzard (2007), it is the inclusion of rules that govern the
player’s behavior that creates a game. This, in turn, creates play
boundaries. These boundaries may be defined by children, or
by adults and in the latter case the games often become com-
petitive, organized sports (Lever, 1978). For the former, how-
ever, this creates child-directed games-with-rules.
Adult involvement in children’s play is often considered to
have a detrimental effect on child directed play (Opie & Opie,
1969). Adult-directed play changes the form and frequency of
child-directed play, often causing it to cease entirely (Schwartz-
man, 1983).
This is not to say that adult directed play has no place in the
life of a child. Adult-directed play is usually supervised by
adults and encompasses all games and play activities introduced
to children by adults, such as team sports and structured games
(Schwartzman, 1983). For this purpose, adults may take on a
number of roles including supervisor, tutor, instructor, coach
and so on. Unlike child-directed play that can occur anywhere
and at anytime, adult-directed play is usually structured in time
and location (Schwartzman, 1983).
Adult-directed play, however, is now more prevalent than
ever before (Power, 2000). In addition, modern society has also
witnessed another shift in direction, as parents become too busy
to supervise and direct their children’s play at all times (Karsten
& Vliet, 2006). Caught in a dilemma between being unable to
supervise their children and not wanting their children to play
unsupervised, there appears, for parents, to be only one solution:
indoor supervised play (Honoré, 2008). This shift towards in-
door play where children can be supervised, but don’t need to
be constantly engaged with, is reinforced by arguments about
increased traffic flow and predatory adults (Mayo & Nairn,
2009) and becomes a shift in parenting styles to a managerial
approach. Honoré (2008) calls this “The Age of the Managed
Child”.
However, structured adult-directed play is also found in
residential outdoor learning settings (LTS, 2010). Here, adult-
directed play allows children to develop understandings and
abilities in co-operation, practice and order, all of which are
valuable skills in developing them as well rounded citizens.
Furthermore, outdoor learning environments, in particular those
which focus on child-centered learning approaches (e.g. forest
schools) increase the opportunities for children to learn and
take appropriate risk within a safe and familiar environment
(O’Brien & Murray, 2007).
Evidence suggests that supporting children to conduct their
own play and learning in the outdoors promotes recreational
activity in childhood that extends into adult life (Gnanapra-
gasam, 2010). This should lead to improved health and wellbe-
ing, whilst allowing children to also engage with nature at their
own pace. Sobel (1993) suggests that this will allow children to
create a connection and ownership with the natural environment.
A possible outlet for these outdoor, child-directed play and
Copyright © 2012 SciRes.
154
F. WOOD ET AL.
games may be found in residential outdoor learning environ-
ments where children are given the time and freedom to con-
duct their own play and learning.
It is clear that the best environments for childhood learning
are those which balance, in yin-yang fashion, different types of
play, individual and group activities, different environments
with different resources for learning, etc. and where children
are encouraged to seek their own learning as well as being re-
assured by enthusiastic and empathetic tutors.
Research Aim
The main of this research study was tounderstand the context,
from a child’s perspective, of learning in the outdoors (in a
residential setting) with a particular focus on the contribution
this experience may have on a child’s preference for learning
and play through healthy physical activity in the outdoors.
Research Approach and Methodology
Methodology
This study was concerned with using a research method that
would uncover participating children’s preferences for physical
play activity in the outdoors. Furthermore, the method should
provide an opportunity to establish the effects, if any, of a short
residential stay in outdoor learning in a before and after study.
Giving children the opportunity to discuss their responses was
an additional desirable aspect of the chosen method. Finally, it
was considered essential that the chosen method should provide
the opportunity for the development of design recommenda-
tions that had been informed by participating children.
Considering these constraints, Personal Construct Psycho-
logy (PCP), a method developed by Kelly (1955) and later re-
fined by Aspinall and Ujam (1992) and Ward-Thompson
(1995), was chosen.
Kelly (1955) believed that human behavior was based on
“individual constructions of reality rather than direct contact
with reality”. Kelly’s principle of PCP is therefore based on the
concept that the human perception and response to reality is
mediated by these constructions (Ward-Thompson, 1995). The
properties of these constructions are elements (objects, events,
places, and people) and constructs (the distinguishing aspects of
the individual aspects, features or qualities).
PCP has been used to elicit design outputs in architectural
design since the early 1970s. Indeed, several designers have
noted its potential and Peled (1976) in particular, used it to
explore aspects of place (Aspinall & Ujam, 1992). Since its
introduction to this field of design, PCP has been used, on oc-
casion, with children acting as active participants in the final
stages of the design process (Aspinall & Ujam, 1992; Ward-
Thompson, 1995). This suggested that PCP might provide
suitable data for analysis and assist in the re-design of outdoor
programmes and activities for implementation in residential
outdoor learning. PCP could then be applied in conjunction
with the closely related method of Ecoanalysis (Peled, 1990).
Eco-analysis is aimed at exposing participants underlying emo-
tional responses to the elements they place on a spatial diagram
(Ward-Thompson, 1995). Peled developed this method in order
to understand human experiences of place in terms of design
and psychotherapy (Aspinall & Ujam, 1992).
Ecoanalysis is based on the notion that we experience the
space around us in the same way that we experience our own
bodies (Aspinall & Ujam, 1992; Ward-Thompson, 1995). It is a
location task which explores the spatial organization in which
participants experience the front, back, left, right and central
aspects of a spatial layout (Figure 1) within which they are
asked to place elements. These specific “locations” relate to the
space around our bodies.
Aspinall and Ujam (1992) claim that an “expressive ana-
logue” is created in the analysis of the participant’s arrange-
ments. This portrays the importance and emotions embodied in
the participant’s spatial ordering. For instance, Peled’s assump-
tions are that when a child is asked a question and responds by
placing a counter on the spatial diagram its location has the
following meaning: 1) In the center implies a wish for integra-
tion; 2) In the front implies the participant would openly state a
desire for this element; 3) In the back implies the participant
may not so openly admit a desired integration of this element; 4)
In the left implies rejection; and 5) In the right implies accep-
tance (Aspinall & Ujam, 1992). This method would allow par-
ticipating children to be involved in the future design of activi-
ties.
To carry out a study using these Projective Methods involves
three stages. Stage One is used to explore the participant’s ho-
listic experience of the site. PCP is used “to draw out aspects of
place experience, activities and artifacts with which the re-
spondents identified in a positive way” (Ward-Thompson, 1995:
p. 126). In this way, participants identify their favored charac-
teristics within the site using a selection of photographs (Figure
2). The use of photographs during PCP sessions with children
has proven successful in previous applications of these pro-
jective methods (Aspinall & Ujam, 1992; Ward-Thompson,
1995).
In Stage Two these aspects are listed as elements and trans-
ferred onto small white counters. Finally, in Stage Three the
participants are involved in an Eco-analysis session where they
place these counters on to a spatial diagram (Figure 1).
Figure 1.
Example of Eco-analysis spatial layout diagram used for the positioning
of element counters. Source: Ward-Thompson, C. (1995) School play-
ground design: a projective approach with pupils and staff. Landscape
Research, 20, 124-140.
Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 155
F. WOOD ET AL.
Participants
The study was conducted with a group of 20 children aged
between 11 and 12 years (ten females and ten males) partici-
pating in a five-day (Monday to Friday inclusive) residential
stay in an outdoor learning centre in Scotland.
Research Procedure
PCP was limited to 15 minutes with each child on Day One
(Monday) and Day Four (Thursday) of their residential stay in
outdoor learning. During this time, the first 10 minutes were
spent conducting a PCP session whilst the remaining 5 minutes
were spent interviewing each child about their play preferences.
An additional period of 30 minutes was also allocated for the
Eco-analysis session on Day Three (Wednesday).
Details of the protocols for the various stages are now de-
scribed.
Stage One—Image Selection (PCP): Each child was pre-
sented with a series of twenty A4 color photographs (Figure 2).
Then each child was asked to imagine they were with their best
friend and to answer the following five questions: 1) What is
your ideal outdoor play environment? Select a photograph that
best shows this; 2) Which three things do you like most about
the photograph you have chosen? 3) What do you not like about
the photograph you have chosen? 4) What would you do there?
and 5) Think of your ideal place of play. How does it differ
from the photograph you have chosen?
Children’s responses to each of these questions were re-
corded for later analysis.
Stage Two—Element Identification and Categorization:
This stage was conducted by one of the authors (FW) without
the involvement of the participants. The responses given during
the first stage generated a number of elements and constructs
associated with outdoor play and learning environments. Dur-
ing this stage, the results from each participating child were
listed and grouped into three categories: 1) Aspects of place
experience e.g. clean, quiet, shaded and so on; 2) Activities e.g.
climbing, running, talking, sitting and so on; and 3) Artifacts
e.g. trees, bushes, dens, seats, buildings and so on.
These elements were then subsequently written onto white
Figure 2.
Photographs shown to participants during Stage One.
counters for use during Stage Three (Eco-analysis). Duplicate
counters were made for each of the elements so that the chil-
dren could place them within the spatial layout more than once.
Stage Three—Eco-Analysis: During this stage only 12 of
the original 20 participating children were available. These 12
children were divided into three small groups of four children.
Each group were given 20 minutes to converse and arrange
the element counters onto the spatial diagram in regards to their
ideal outdoor play and learning environments. Three identical
sets of element counters had been created for this purpose—one
for each of the participating groups.
The final layouts of each group were noted. These layouts
were later replicated, using an electronic drawing software
package, for subsequent analysis.
Stage Four—Play Images: In Stage Four additional time
was spent with each child at the start and end of their residential
stay in order to ascertain their play preferences. During these
sessions, each participating child was presented with a series of
twenty A4 color photographs representing various scenes of
play (Figure 3).
On Day 1, these images were laid out in front of each child
and they were then asked to: 1) Think of your usual play acti-
vity. Select a photograph which best shows this; 2) Think of
your ideal play activity. Select a photograph that best shows
this.
On Day 4, the same images were laid out in front of each
child, however this time only Question 2 was asked.
Children’s responses to each of these questions were re-
corded verbatim.
Limitations of the Study
The small number of participants who were available to be
involved in the study and had the necessary ethical consent,
reduced the amount of statistical analysis that could be per-
formed.
The outdoor centre in which the study was being conducted
determined the timing of events and the availability of partici-
pants. No pilot study was conducted to test the research ap-
proach. As such, the same form of analysis used by Aspinall and
Figure 3.
Photographs shown to participants during Stage Four.
Copyright © 2012 SciRes.
156
F. WOOD ET AL.
Ujam (1992) and Ward-Thompson (1995) was applied and
followed accordingly.
Furthermore, an application of a method that uses images
adds another level of ambiguity to the interpretation of the data.
Therefore, had a pilot study been conducted, this may have
improved the selection of photographs for the main study.
The placing of elements on the spatial layout furthered the
ambiguity of the data gathered. Indeed, it was unclear if the
children were following the method of mapping out their ideal
outdoor play environment or, alternatively, if they were just
trying to fill all areas of the diagram.
Generating a list of elements from children’s verbal re-
sponses also proved difficult. Their vocal responses varied from
grunts to babble and their vocabulary was limited. Furthermore,
differentiating between elements of artifacts, activities and ex-
perience of place proved difficult when, of course, these are all
interwoven and connected. Artifacts, for example, tend to de-
fine the activities that occur in spaces, thus making them into
places.
Data Analysis and Results
Stage One—Image Selection (PCP): Responses to Q1 (see
p. 4) were analyzed in relation to the most popular (top 70%),
marginally popular (next 30%) and least popular (0%).
Figure 4 shows the rank order of the total selection of these
photographs by boys and girls.
Stage Two—Element Identification: Responses to Ques-
tions 2-5 (see p. 4) were analysed, by taking only the responses
given by the fourteen children who chose the four most popular
photographs in response to Question 1.
Although an identical interview procedure was followed for
each child, it should be noted that, in some cases, when re-
sponding to Question 2, children indicated more than three
things, only two things, or sometimes nothing at all. However,
all elements discussed were listed even in cases where a child
mentioned the same element more than once.
The process identified 25 elements liked and 5 elements dis-
liked, in addition to 12 elements of ideal places to play, in re-
sponse to these questions.
Stage Two—Element Categorization: The elements gather-
ed from the participant responses to Q2 and Q4 were catego-
rized according to one of three categories: 1) an Experience of
Place; 2) an Activity or; 3) an Artifact (Table 1).
Table 2 indicates that Activities were the most frequently
chosen category, followed by Experience of Place, and then,
finally, Artifacts. It also shows that Photograph no. 1 (splashing)
had the most responses across all categories whilst Photograph
no. 8 (rock climbing) had the least. Photograph no. 8 was also
the only photograph that did not contribute to any Artifact
category, but was the highest contributor to the Activity cate-
gory.
Stage Three—Eco-Analysis: This section illustrates the
children’s placement of the categorized elements (identified in
Stage Two) onto an Eco-analysis diagram.
Figure 5 shows the aggregate data from all three groups of
children involved in this process.
The three element categories (activity, artifact and experi-
ence of place) are distinguished through the use of different
colors: 1) Activity—Blue; 2) Place—Orange; and 3) Arti-
fact—Pink.
This shows that Artifact elements were by far the most
0
1
2
3
4
1 19 813 4 17 310 25679 11121415161820
Number of Times Chosen
Photograph Iden tification Num ber
Girls
Boys
Figure 4.
Rank order of selections of all photograph choices in re-
sponse to Question 1 (n = 20).
Table 1.
Categorisation of elements identified in response to Questions 2 and 4
with regard to the four most popular photographs n = 14.
Element Category Element Category
Colour Place Walk Activity
Nice Views Place Look(ing) Activity
Fun Place Paddling Activity
Nice Place Sit Activity
Nice Scenery Place Shout Activity
Cute Place Splash Activity
High/Height Place Play Activity
Steep Place Water Artefact
Sunny Place Animals Homes Artefact
Adventure Place Rocks Artefact
Peaceful Place Garden Artefact
Nice Place Place Forest Artefact
Touch Animal Activity Leaves Artefact
Picnic Activity Bike Artefact
(Make a) Den Activity Animals Artefact
popular choice for location on the spatial diagram. In addition,
this also shows that all three of the element categories were
distributed across the entire spatial layout.
Stage Four—Play Images: Responses to Q1 (see p. 4) were
analyzed according to the rank order of the total selection of
photographs by boys and girls (Figure 6).
Responses to Q2 “Think of your ideal play activity. Select a
photograph which best shows this” were analyzed according to
the total number of times each photograph was chosen for each
gender, before and after their residential stay (Figure 7 and
Tables 3 and 4). To establish whether there was a significant
gender associated difference in choice of indoor or outdoor play
at the start and end of their residential period, differences for
the paired data were then calculated and a sign test conducted.
Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 157
F. WOOD ET AL.
Copyright © 2012 SciRes.
158
Figure 5.
Participant’s element placement during Stage Two (for total number of respondents n = 12).
Tables 3 and 4 show that both genders demonstrated a pre-
ference for outdoor play at the start of their residential stay in
outdoor learning. However, due to the subjective nature of a
research method using images, no assumptions could be made
about the symmetry or normality of the data, so a nonparamet-
ric sign test was conducted on these results to determine if ob-
served differences could be attributed to the residential stay.
The further away from 0 the mean difference was, the more
likely it was that the play preferences had changed after chil-
dren had experienced a residential stay.
Table 2.
Categorisation of all elements identified as being liked about the four
most popular photographs during Questions 2 and 4 with regard to
individual photograph choice (n = 14).
Categories Place Activities Artifacts
Total Times
Mentioned
Photo 1 (4)
6 9 5 20
Photo 19 (4)
7 6 1 14
Photo 8 (3)
2 11 0 13
Photo 13 (3)
7 5 2 14
Total Times
Mentioned 22 31 8 61
The null hypothesis was that no difference between the sam-
ples would be observed (m0 = 0). Any differences equal to m0
were removed from the sample and the number of positive re-
sults was counted. This number is the test statistic and its null
distribution is B(n,1/2) where n is the sample size (after re-
moving any values equal to m0). This test was conducted using
Minitab software (Table 5).
Table 5 shows there is little evidence to refute the null hy-
pothesis for boys, but some evidence to challenge the null hy-
pothesis for girls. However, the data suggests that neither gen-
der demonstrated a significant difference in their change of play
preferences towards outdoor play after their residential stay in
outdoor learning.
Discussion
PCP and Eco- AnalysisDiscussion of Methods
The use of PCP and Eco-analysis in the context of this study
F. WOOD ET AL.
0
1
2
3
4
917361281941812571011131415161820
Number of Times Chosen
Photograph Identification Num ber
Girls
Boys
Figure 6.
Rank order of selections of all photograph choices in re-
sponse to Question 1 (n = 20).
0
1
2
3
4
317812710191 41661518202 5 9111314
Number of Times Chosen
Photogr aph Iden tification N umb er
Day
One
Day
Four
Figure 7.
Response rates of all photograph choices in response to
Question 2 on Days 1 and 4 (n = 20).
Table 3.
Response rate of indoor play photograph choices before and after, girls
and boys (n = 20).
Girls “Indoor Play”
Photograph Choices in
Response to Q2
Boys “Indoor Play”
Photograph Choices in
Response to Q2
Day 1 Day 4 Day 1 -
Day 4 Day 1 Day 4 Day 1 -
Day 4
Photo 2 0 0 0 0 0 0
Photo 4 0 0 0 0 2 2
Photo 6 0 0 0 0 1 1
Photo 8 1 3 2 1 0 1
Photo 9 0 0 0 0 0 0
Photo 11 0 0 0 0 0 0
Photo 12 1 3 2 0 0 0
Photo 13 0 0 0 0 0 0
Photo 15 0 1 1 0 0 0
Photo 18 0 0 0 1 0 1
Sum 2 7 5 2 3 1
Mean 0.5 0.1
Table 4.
Response rate of outdoor play photograph choices before and after,
girls and boys (n = 20).
Girls “Outdoor Play”
Photograph Choices in
Response to Q2
Boys “Outdoor Play”
Photograph Choices in
Response to Q2
Day 1
Day
4
Day 1 -
Day 4 Day 1 Day
4
Day 1 -
Day 4
Photo 1 2 0 2 0 0 0
Photo 3 1 0 1 3 2 1
Photo 5 0 0 0 0 0 0
Photo 7 2 1 1 0 0 0
Photo 101 0 1 2 0 2
Photo 140 0 0 0 0 0
Photo 161 0 1 0 1 1
Photo 170 0 0 3 3 0
Photo 191 2 1 0 0 0
Photo 200 0 0 0 1 1
Sum 8 3 5 8 7 1
Mean 0.5 0.1
Table 5.
Results of a nonparametric sign test on the data for girls and boys in
response to Question 2.
N BelowEqual Above P Median
Difference
Indoor Girls 10 3 7 0 0.25000.000
Difference
Outdoor Girls10 1 4 5 0.21880.500
Difference Indoor
Boys 10 2 6 2 1.00000.000
Difference
Outdoor Boys10 3 6 1 0.62500.000
proved to be useful in considering the redesign of programmes
and activities for children’s outdoor play and learning. Firstly,
these techniques are designed to help participants to identify
their desires by tapping into their subconscious responses. Eiser
(1986) refers to these as “pre-conscious” factors. These are
especially important when designing with and for children who
may have different and opposing views to those of adults. So,
these methods allowed participating children’s unconscious
preference for different activities and artifacts within an out-
door learning environment to be uncovered. Secondly, the pro-
jective methods used during this study had been applied pre-
viously as a means for involving children in the design process
of both playgrounds (Ward-Thompson, 1995) and landscape
architecture (Aspinall & Ujam, 1992). Thus, little adaptation
was required for application in this study. Finally, these projec-
tive methods allowed participating children to act as key stake-
holders in the design phase of this study.
Eco-Analysi s and PCP Discussion of Findings
The analyses of participant’s location of elements revealed
some interesting results. The children demonstrated a desire to
Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 159
F. WOOD ET AL.
integrate all 3 categories of the elements presented to them. In
particular, artifacts seemed to attract greater interest than activi-
ties. Indeed, in terms of total numbers, artifact elements were
placed more frequently on the spatial layouts than activity or
place elements. However, artifact elements were also placed
more often in the outer parts of the spatial layout than in the
inner. This suggests that, despite being the most frequently
located elements, they may not have been as important as some
of the other elements. Indeed, activity elements (building a den,
splashing, shouting, playing, exploring, laughing, touching
animals, look, sitting and picnicking) were placed in the centre
spaces more frequently than the artifact elements (trees, ani-
mals, nature, garden, rocks, wood, animals home, friends, water,
leaves and bike).
However, each of the artifact elements placed in the centre
spaces of the spatial layout (laugh, shout, build dens and touch
animals) all raised possibilities for an activity.
Indeed, it would seem that artifacts and activities are interre-
lated and difficult to separate, especially in the eyes of a child.
Therefore, aspects of activity, in addition to artifacts that pre-
sent possibilities for activity (e.g. rocks to climb), may attract
greater interest from children than artifacts that do not seem to
offer the same potential for activities. This will be of impor-
tance when making recommendations for outdoor learning
environments.
Interestingly when placing experience elements in the centre
of the spatial layout, the children demonstrated a high prefer-
ence for the place’s sense of peace, color, height, views and just
a nice place. This suggests that, despite parental beliefs regard-
ing the boisterous nature of young children, they also appear to
desire places for reflection and quiet, much in the same way
that adults do. The choice of such places, in particular, in an
outdoor learning environment, would add support to the notion
of experiential learning and the need to allow children the ap-
propriate spaces in which to reflect on their learning experi-
ences (Beard & Wilson, 2006).
A closer look at the elements placed on the spatial layouts
highlights that trees were also extremely popular with this co-
hort of children. This may be for two reasons. First, children,
especially after spending time in residential outdoor learning,
may view trees as “fun” things to climb; after all they have just
participated in a “Tree Climb” activity during their residential
stay. Second, however, it may be that these children are re-
sponding to a tendency towards an association with trees (bio-
philia). Indeed, other examples of children’s tendencies towards
nature were also found; with the most frequently positioned
elements by all three groups being leaves, trees and animals.
These children appeared to demonstrate a desire for the oppor-
tunity to engage with nature.
Elements relating specifically to shelter also seemed to be of
particular importance. For instance, trees, building a den, sitting
and picnicking were all popular central elements. This may be
because these children (aged 7 - 12 years) were at a stage in
their childhood where their desire for creating small worlds for
themselves, out with the confines of their family home, is de-
veloping (Sobel, 1996).
It is interesting to also note that the children’ dislikes ap-
peared to be predominantly safety orientated, ranging from
worries about deep or shallow water and its temperature, to the
height of artifacts within their play spaces. Gill (2007) high-
lights this child-oriented risk-aversion as a rising problem in
modern society, resulting from too much adult supervision and
adult-directed play.
Play PreferencesDiscussion of Findings
In response to the question about their usual play activity,
participating children selected a wide range of photographs. Of
the 9 examples of usual play activity selected, the most popular
choices was playing with a family pet and playing football.
Whilst the former was chosen by boys and girls equally, the
latter was chosen predominantly by boys. It is interesting, al-
though perhaps not surprising, that playing with a family pet
would be such a popular choice. Indeed, with children having
increasingly less opportunities to play outside, unsupervised
with friends, parents working increasingly long hours and one
child families becoming more common, pets have, unsurpris-
ingly, become the play partner of many modern children.
Playing football, or indeed any sport, was also not a surpris-
ing popular choice for the usual play of participating children.
Neither was it surprising that this activity was also chosen more
frequently by boys than by girls (Hughes, 1999).
Riding a bike and watching television were also equally rated.
This time girls demonstrated a preference for watching televi-
sion, and boys for riding a bike. This is also in line with evi-
dence in the literature which states that girls are more likely to
engage in sedentary activities (Lever, 1978), whilst boys will
engage in more robust or active play (Power, 2000).
Only a further four activities were chosen by children as rep-
resenting their usual play. Each of these activities was chosen
by only one child: listening to music and playing in rivers were
chosen by girls and playing board games and playing the Xbox
were chosen by boys.
A further eleven activities were not chosen by any children
as examples of their usual play. These were: rollerblading,
building dens, playing with fish, trampolining, reading, climb-
ing trees, indoor gardening, walking, playing with frogs, fishing
and looking at insects. It is disappointing that so many of these
unselected activities were those that would usually occur in the
outdoors. Indeed, only one example of outdoor natural play had
been chosen (playing in rivers) and by only one child, who,
contrary to evidence in the literature, had been female. With
only seven children selecting activities that were even remotely
physically active, the results suggest that the remaining partici-
pating children find passive play adequate in terms of general
satisfaction but hardly enough in terms of healthy, physical
activity.
Whilst these results support the evidence in the literature, and
appear to confirm that children’s play is, in fact, moving inside
in favor of often electronic activities, it is both unsettling and
surprising that this appeared to be what children wanted. Fur-
ther information from this cohort was now needed to establish
whether the play activities described were the choice of the
children, or whether other contributing factors influenced their
usual play. This was established by asking the same children
about their ideal play.
By far the most favored ideal play activity was riding a bike
and was chosen predominantly by boys. Building dens and
playing football were also highly favored and again chosen
predominantly by boys. This is not surprising considering, if
given the opportunity, boys often favor energetic, physical ac-
tivity in outdoor environments. The two most popular activities
described by girls were trampolining and climbing trees. Whilst
trampolining, occurring in family gardens, was not a surprising
Copyright © 2012 SciRes.
160
F. WOOD ET AL.
choice for girls, climbing trees is a much more robust activity
and would have been expected, based on the evidence in the
literature, to have been more desirable by boys.
Conclusion
Despite the intriguing possibility and idealistic, almost ro-
mantic proposal that all children’s learning could occur out-
doors the reality is that indoor and outdoor learning must act
together (cooperate) in a complimentary fashion and are not
substitutes for one another (LTS, 2010).
Learning itself is a deep and pleasurable urge which pro-
motes in young people a feeling of fulfillment and great happi-
ness when engaged in challenging, non-trivial pursuits (Haidt,
2006). For children, play is a prerequisite to enjoyable self-
learning and creative problem solving. Furthermore, self-learn-
ing conducted in the outdoors is more “real” than the controlled
environment of the indoor classroom. It presents the child with
more opportunities for choice (because of greater novelty) and
more self-discovered challenges (because of greater complexity)
in which the learner needs to take responsibility for their own
learning and to face the consequences of their own decision
making.
Children want to be happy and their parents want them to be
happy too. As Guy Claxton says (2008) “they (parents) want
them (their children) to be safe, healthy independent people
who feel at home in the world; (author’s emphasis) who can
make and keep friends; who can face what life throws at them
intelligently and cheerfully. Most parents are less worried about
what form their children’s lives take than about their mental
and emotional wellbeing. They know that happiness has to do
with purpose and passion rather than positivity: with the satis-
faction of taking on real challenges and not running away from
difficulty” (p. 14). Play is the ideal tool for children’s learning.
As discussed previously, children’s play can be broken down
into two clear domains; Adult-Directed and Child-Directed.
Within each of these domains children may engage in indoor or
outdoor play. A unique attribute of Child-Directed Play is fan-
tasy or imaginative play, in which children create their own
scenarios and environments. Games are also a common feature
of play. In child directed games, children learn how to make
rules, through which they learn the art of negotiation. In adult
directed games, children learn how to obey rules, and the notion
of competition is introduced. In these settings, games can
quickly change to sports. This occurs when children feel in-
creasingly less responsible for the type of play in which they
are engaged, and instead become a participant in a “training”
activity. In this sense, all Adult-Directed Play can become
training. That is, as soon as a child stops enjoying the activity it
changes from a “play” activity to a “learning” activity. This is
because, whilst play is an effective learning tool, enjoyment
remains a clear requirement of play and so, as soon as an acti-
vity ceases to be enjoyed by a child it can no longer be termed
Adult-Directed Play, becoming instead Adult-Directed Train-
ing.
The role of child-directed or free-play is encouraged in the
literature, especially when it occurs in out-of-doors environ-
ments. Outdoor, natural environments are particularly ideal as
are able to satisfy the creative needs of a child (Maudsley,
2007). Garvey (1977) also confirms that participation in play
allows children to develop enhanced motor skills, respond to
different levels of aggression and learn how to interact and
cooperate with others.
However, whether this learning will have a long-term effect
on these children remains unclear. Indeed, whilst Hattie et al.
(1997) and Sobel (1996) both conclude that residential outdoor
programmes can have a long lasting effect on the lives and
future environmental commitment of participants, Mittelstaedt,
et al. (1999) suggest a residential experience in outdoor educa-
tion will only reinforce a pre-existing appreciation for the natu-
ral environment. This study suggests that if you are looking for
a change in the children’s attitudes, as reflected in their choice
of indoor/outdoor play, for example, five days in residential
outdoor learning environments has little or no effect. But, if a
major benefit to many of the children is to get away from
school and the continuous overview of their parents, to be with
other children (peers) and to have an exciting time, then the
effect is significant. The lasting effect of even that is immeas-
urable. How many people who 50 years ago experienced a stay
in residential outdoor learning can clearly identify the benefits
they got from the experience? To understand these issues better
would require either a large retrospective survey or a longitu-
dinal study.
In parallel to this research two of the authors (SB and FW)
conducted a small review of historical children’s games, many
of which held the desired characteristics of child-directed
physical activities conducted in outdoor settings. In particular,
the game “Sappy Soddgers” was reviewed. According to Opie
& Opie (1969) this game is classified as a chasing game, col-
lectively described as “three lives”.
The aim of the game was to end up with one child as the
victor. The game could start with 6 to 16 or more players. The
game began with all the players standing in a circle, legs apart
and feet touching those of the adjacent players. A ball was
thrown in the air in the centre of the circle and the first
“thrower” was identified by whose legs the ball went through in
coming to a halt. When the thrower was identified everyone
else scattered as quickly as possible. Thereafter the ball was
always “in play” being thrown by a “thrower” with the inten-
tion of hitting another player on the clothed part of the body. A
player hit three times was deemed “dead” and given one last
free throw which, if successful in hitting another player, led to
the resurrection of the original “victim”. There was no referee
or umpire and any disputes, beyond these pre-defined rules,
were resolved by the players. Such resolutions only lasted for
the duration of the game being played. The game ended with a
single “victor” or when conditions prevented further play—bad
weather, darkness or a lost ball. The equipment consisted of
only a ball, usually an old, discarded tennis ball with no rough,
surface skin.
Despite the conditions that led to the demise of the original
game, its resurrection in residential outdoor learning settings
could restore benefits to childhood health and sociality through
physical play activity. To do so in a modern society will proba-
bly require making the game more fashionable: involving new
technologies and styling and indeed adding to the cost of pre-
paring the child for the game. This, of course, is no different
from any other modern versions of games and sports. In this
particular sport, the aim, as with the original game, would be to
reduce the overall number of players so as to arrive at a winner.
In this way, children would learn about co-operation and com-
petition. Such a game would develop some cognitive capacity
as well as physical skills. Of course, it would be desired that the
original game be resurrected in its original form and for it to be
Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 161
F. WOOD ET AL.
Copyright © 2012 SciRes.
162
played and modified by children for children and for it to re-
main as much “child-directed play” as possible so that it is
filled with enjoyment and friendship.
It is hoped that the transformation of this original game may
encourage the re-design of other old games which would facili-
tate the re-enchantment of active play and games in outdoor
settings with the overall intention of stimulating exercise and
enjoyment in voluntary, child-directed play activities.
This brief example demonstrates how the context of self-di-
rected physical activities might be incorporated into residential
outdoor learning activities for the benefit of children’s in-
creased health and wellbeing through self-directed exploration
and play.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to acknowledge the contributions of
staff from both Scottish Outdoor Education Centers (SOEC) and
Duncan of Jordanstone College of Art and Design (DJCAD),
University of Dundee, Dundee, Scotland for aspects of this
study.
REFERENCES
Adler, P. A., & Adler, P. (1998). Peer power. New York: Rutgers Uni-
versity Press.
Ames, M. (2007). Going postal: Rage, murder, and rebellion: From
reagan’s workplaces to Clinton’s columbine and beyond. New York:
Soft Skull Press.
Aspinall, A., & Ujam, F. (1992). A projective approach to designing
with children. Landscape Research , 17, 124-131.
doi:10.1080/01426399208706375
Asthana, A., & Revill, J. (2008). Is it time to let children play outdoors
once more? The Observer.
Beard, C., & Wilson, J. P. (2006). Experiential learning second edition:
A best practice handbook for educators and trainers. London: Kogan
Page Ltd.
Claxton, G. (2008). Wha t ’s the point of school? Redisc o v e ring the heart
of education. Oxford: One World Publications.
Dansky, J. L. (1999). Play. In M. A Runco, & S. R. Pritzken (Eds.),
Encyclopaedia of creativity (pp. 393-408). London: Academic Press.
Eiser, J. (1986). Social psychology: Attitudes, cognition and social
behaviour (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Faculty of Public Health (FPH) (2010). Great outdoors: How our natu-
ral health service uses green space to promote well being. London:
Faculty of Public Health.
Garvey, C. (1977). Play. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Gazzard, A. (2007). Playing in a virtual world: Exploration and aspects
of play. Proceedings of 17th International Conference on Artificial
Reality and Telexistence, 288-289. doi:10.1109/ICAT.2007.27
Gill, T. (2007). No fear: Growing up in a risk averse society. Lisbon:
Calouste Gulbenkian Foundation.
Gill, T. (2010). Nothing ventured. Balancing risk and benefits in the
outdoors. Lifton: English Outdoor Council.
Gnanapragasam, A. (2010). Reorientating education towards sustain-
ability: Promoting ecological literacy through outdoor learning in
Scotland. Masters Thesis, St. Andrew’s: University of St. Andrew’s.
Haidt, J. (2006). The happiness hypothesis: Finding modern truth in
ancient wisdom. New York: Basic Books.
Hattie, J. A., Neill, J. T., & Richards, G. E. (1997). Adventure educa-
tion and outward bound: Out-of-class experiences that make a lasting
difference. Review of Education al Research, 67, 43-87.
Holloway, S. L., & Valentine, G. (2000). Children’s geographies, play-
ing, living, learning. London: Routeledge.
Honoré, C. (2008). Under pressure: Rescuing our children from the
culture of hyper-parentin g . London: Orion Books.
Hughes, F. P. (1999). Children, play and development. Boston, MA:
Allyn and Bacon.
James, A., Jenks, C., & Prout, A. (1998). Theorizing childhood. Oxford:
Blackwell Publishers Ltd.
Karsten, L., & Vliet, W. V. (2006). Increasing children’s freedom of
movement: Introduction. Children, Youth and Environments, 16, 69-
73.
Kelly, G. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York:
Norton.
Learning and Teaching Scotland (LTS) (2007). Taking learning out-
doors, partnerships for excellence. Glasgow: Learning and Teaching
Scotland.
Learning and Teaching Scotland (LTS) (2010). Curriculum for excel-
lence through Outdoor Learning. Glasgow: Learning and Teaching
Scotland.
Lever, J. (1978). Sex differences in the complexity of children’s play
and games. Amer ican Sociological Re v i ew, 43, 471-483.
doi:10.2307/2094773
Louv, R. (2005). Last child in the woods: Saving our children from
nature-deficit disorder. London: Atlantic Books.
Maudsley, M. (2007). Children’s play in natural environments. Chil-
dren’s Play Information Service: National Children’s Bureau, UK.
Mayo, E., & Nairn, A. (2009). Consumer kids: How big business is
grooming our children for profit. London: Constable and Robinson
Ltd.
Minister of Education (2004). Outdoor connections. London: Minister
of Education.
Mittelstaedt, R., Sanker, L., & Vanderveer, B. (1999). Impact of a
week-long experiential education program on environmental attitude
and awareness. Journal of Experien tial Education, 22, 138-148.
O’brien, L., & Murray, R. (2007). Forest school and its impacts on
young children. Case studies in Britain Urban Forestry and Urban
Greening, 6, 249-265.
Opie, I., & Opie, P. (1969). Children’s games in street and playground.
Oxford, MA: Oxford University Press.
Petersen, A. F. (1988). Why children and young animals play: A new
theory of play and its role in problem solving. Copenhagen: Munks-
gaard.
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York:
Norton.
Power, T. (2000). Play and exploration in children and animals. Lon-
don: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Quart, A. (2006). Hothouse kids: The dilemma of the gifted child. City
of Westminster: Penguin Group.
Roussou, M. (2004). Learning by doing and learning through play: An
exploration of interactivity in virtual environments for children.
Computers in Entertai nment, 2, 10. doi:10.1145/973801.973818
Schwartzman, H. B. (1983). Child-structured play. In F. E. Manning
(Ed.), Association for the anthropological study of play (pp. 200-
214). New York: Leisure Press.
Schlumberger Excellence in Educational Development SEED (2004). A
curriculum for excellence. Edinburgh: Schlumberger Excellence in
Educational Development.
Sobel, D. (1993). Children’s special places: Exploring the role of forts,
dens, and bush houses in mid dle ch il dho od. Zephyr Press, USA.
Sobel, D. (1996). Beyond ecophobia. Great Barrington: The Orion
Society.
Ward-Thompson, C. (1995). School playground design: A projective
approach with pupils and staff. Landscape Research, 20, 124-140.
doi:10.1080/01426399508706466
West, J. (1996). Child centered play therapy. London: Arnold.