 Advances in Physical Education 2012. Vol.2, No.3, 110-118 Published Online August 2012 in SciRes (http://www.SciRP.org/journal/ape) http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/ape.2012.23020 Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 110 Advancing Task Involvement, Intrinsic Motivation and Metacognitive Regulation in Physical Education Classes: The Self-Check Style of Teaching Makes a Difference Athanasios Papaioannou*, Argiris Theodosiou, Marina Pashali, Nikolaos Digelidis Department of Physical Education and Sport Science, University of Thessaly, Trikala, Greece Email: *sakispap@pe.uth.gr Received March 12th, 2012; revised April 14th, 2012; accepted April 30th, 2012 It was hypothesized that “self-check” style of teaching would be more preferable in terms of creating a mastery-oriented climate, and promoting adaptive achievement goals, intrinsic motivation and metacogni- tive activity in physical education classes. Two hundred seventy-nine (N = 269) 6-grade students were randomly divided into two groups that were taught four consecutive physical education lessons of the same content following either “practice” or “self-check” styles of teaching respectively. Students responded on questionnaires prior and after the intervention. Results revealed significant interactions between groups and measurements. Students in the “self-check” style group scored higher in scales measuring mastery-oriented climate, mastery goal, intrinsic motivation and metacognitive processes and lower in scales measuring performance-goals and performance-oriented motivational climate. These results underscore the importance of using styles of teaching that enhance opportunities for deep cognitive processing and promote mastery-goals and mastery-oriented climates. Keywords: Self-Check; Achievement Goals; Metacognition Introduction The term “teaching style” was firstly established by Mosston (1966), who organized all the teaching methods depending on whether the decisions in a teaching/learning event are made by: the teacher or the student. Later on, together with Ashworth (Mosston & Ashworth, 1986, 1994, 2002) he proposed “the spectrum of teaching styles” on which eleven teaching methods were classified starting with the “command” style (where all the decisions are made by the teacher) and ending with the “self-teaching” style (where all the decisions are made by the student). Several studies used the spectrum in order to evaluate different styles of teaching and their impact on motor skills acquisition (e.g. Abd Al-Salam, 2004; AlMulla-Abdullah, 2003; Beckett, 1990; Boyce, 1992; Goldberger & Gerney, 1986; Gold- berger, Gerney, & Chamberlain, 1982), while other studies ex- plored changes on learner’s motivational variables (Chatoupis & Emmanouel, 2003; Goudas, Biddle, Fox, & Underwood, 1995; Morgan, Kingston, & Sproule, 2005). The present study follows the second line of research by in- vestigating the effects of “self-check” and “practice” teaching styles on students’ cognitions and intrinsic motivation in physical education. Two accepted theoretical frameworks in educational psychology were used. Firstly, according to achievement goal theory (Ames, 1984; Ames & Archer, 1988; Dweck & Legget, 1988; Nicholls, 1984, 1989), in achievement settings such as physical education, two major types of goals predominate. When performance goals are adopted individuals judge success or fail- ure in comparison to others. They feel successful if they outper- form others or surpass a high normative performance aiming to demonstrate evidence of high ability or to avoid showing evi- dence of low ability. When mastery goals are adopted individu- als judge success against with their previous performance and feel successful when they manage to achieve the task and gain ability. As a result, mastery-oriented students are not afraid of facing challenging tasks. Instead, they are often intrinsically motivated in learning conditions, and this is even more profound when they can adjust task challenge to their level of abilities. In such cases mastery-oriented students apply high effort to master a task whereas learning and personal improvement is their ul- timate goal. An additional distinction of these two types of achievement goals was made by Elliot (1999) who suggested that it is also important to explore whether individuals focus on reaching a positive desirable possibility (approach goal) or on avoiding a negative undesirable possibility (avoidance goal). Combining these two dimensions with task- and ego-goals, Elliot (1999) proposed a new framework with four dimensions: performance- approach goal (individuals try to indicate high normative abil- ity), performance-avoidance goal (individuals try to avoid show- ing evidence of low ability), mastery-approach goal (individu- als focus on improving personal competence on a task) and mastery goal (individuals focus on avoiding to perform poorly regarding their personal standards). These differences in people’s goals and corresponding behave- iors may derive from previous experiences (Stipek & Hoffman, 1980), family or social influences (Ames & Archer, 1987; Gott- fried, Fleming, & Gottfried, 1994, Papaioannou, Ampatzoglou, Kalogiannis, & Sagovits, 2008; Parsons, Adler, & Kaczala, 1982; Xiang, McBride, & Bruene, 2003), or teachers’ behavior (Carr & Weigand, 2002; Marshall & Weistein, 1986; Viciana, Cervello, & Ramirez-Lechga, 2007; Weinstein & Middlestadt, 1979). In particular, teachers’ way of instruction, evaluation, type of feed- *Corresponding author.
 A. PAPAIOANNOU ET AL. back and the tasks chosen for teaching, are important dimensions of class structure (Epstein, 1989), which determine students per- ceptions of their class motivational climate as being high or low mastery-oriented and high or low performance-oriented (Ames, 1992). Overall (Dweck & Legget, 1988; Ford, Smith, Weissbein, Gully, & Salas, 1998; Nicholls, 1984, 1989; Pintrich, 2000; Vrugt & Oort, 2008), it is well established that performance goals and performance climates—with the exception of performance-ap- proach goals in particular circumstances (Hidi & Haravkiewicz, 2000)—produce maladaptive behaviours such as surface proc- essing, shallow learning, and use of self-handicapping strategies. On the contrary, mastery-goals and mastery-oriented climates produce adaptive behaviors, emotions and cognitions, such as deep processing (Pintrich & Garcia, 1991; Thill & Brunel, 1995), enjoyment (Cunningham & Xiang, 2008), persistence on effort (Guan, Xiang, McBride, & Bruene, 2006; McCarthy, Jones, & Clark-Carter, 2008) and self-regulated learning (Duda, Cumming, & Balaguer, 2005). The general framework of self-regulation and the more spe- cific theory of metacognition (Brown, 1987; Flavell, 1979) con- stituted the second theoretical background of this study. Self- regulated learners are supposed to act autonomously, guiding their learning processes towards their goals, transferring previ- ous knowledge to related situations, monitoring their progress continuously and adjusting their strategies in the face of diffi- culties. Self-regulation is largely a cognitive process, entails the interaction of personal and environmental variables and as a result differentiates between people of different experience (Ban- dura, 1986; 1997; Carver & Scheier, 1998; Crews, 1993; Kir- schenbaum, 1984; 1987; Kirschenbaum & Wittrock, 1984; Pet- lichkoff, 2003; Zimmerman, 1986). According to Efklides (2001) and others (e.g. Chen & Singer, 1992; Ommundsen, 2006; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990; Vrugt & Oort, 2008) an important element of self-regulation is metacog- nition. Although more theoretical research is necessary to achieve an inclusive definition of metacognition (Veenman, Van Hout- Wolters, & Afflerbach, 2006), literature on this field mainly identifies three strongly intercorrelated features of metacogni- tion: knowledge of cognition, regulation of cognition and meta- cognitive experiences (Brown, 1987; Flavell, 1979; Jacobs & Paris, 1987; Otero & Campanario, 1992; Veenman, Van Hout- Wolters, & Afflerbach, 2006). Knowledge of cognition refers to a person’s awareness about his/her strengths and weaknesses, awareness about the interplay between personal factors and the demands of the new situation and awareness about the usage of the strategies he/she pos- sesses to solve a learning problem. Cross and Paris (1988) men- tioned three kinds of knowledge of cognition: declarative knowl- edge (knowledge about the self and personal strategies), proce- dural knowledge (knowledge about how to use personal strate- gies) and conditional knowledge (knowledge about when and why to use these strategies). However, it is essential to stress the importance of the regu- lating these cognition processes. Regulation of cognition con- sists of the activities/skills which the person uses to control his/her learning, such as: management of the available informa- tion, planning, self-monitoring, strategies for solving an on-line problem and evaluation of the learning products or the strate- gies that had been used (Brown, 1987). Learning experiences are always related with cognitive proc- esses and person’s metacognitive skills developed or facilitated through appropriate teaching strategies. Metacognitive experi- ences refer to the feelings the person experiences in relation to a particular learning task and judgments regarding the process- ing of his/her learning, such as feeling of difficulty, feeling of familiarity, feeling of how well he/she is doing, etc. (Efklides, 2001). Studies in sport and physical education confirmed that self- regulation and metacognition have strong association with mas- tery goals and mastery-oriented climates (Gano-Overway, 2008; Ommundsen, 2003; 2006; Papaioannou, Simou, Kosmidou, Milo- sis, & Tsigilis, 2009; Solmon & Boone, 1993; Solmon & Lee, 1997; Theodosiou, 2004; Theodosiou & Papaioannou, 2006; Theodosiou, Mantis, & Papaioannou, 2008). The results of these studies supported the assumption of educational psychologists (Ames & Archer, 1988; Boekaerts, 1997; Vrugt & Oort, 2008) that the pursuit of mastery goals creates the appropriate sub- stratum for the establishment of metacognition and self-regulation; however, these studies do not allow inferences about causality. For example, as self-regulated learning is ubiquitous in educa- tional research nowadays, specific training programmes which encourage self-regulated learning have been found to be very helpful for students’ learning in academic domain (Loyens, Joshua, & Rikers, 2008). Such kind of research and especially field- based studies in the domain of physical education are required to draw conclusions about the appropriate instructional formats which ensure the development of students’ self-regulatory strate- gies. Research Hypothesis Based on the main characteristics of the “self-check” style of teaching one can hypothesize that learners are facilitated in setting process goals (e.g. definition of process, Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 1996, 1997). One of the main characteristics of this style of teaching is learners’ self-assessment through criteria provided by the teacher (Mosston & Ashworth, 2002). In par- ticular, the physical education teacher prepares criteria sheets for the students, regarding the motor skill that has to be learned. Through these criteria sheets the learners become aware of the core components of the motor skill and they are able to judge the accuracy of their practice giving themselves personal feed- back for every trial or for a set of trials. In addition, increased opportunities are offered to pupils to learn how to regulate their learning by monitoring their progress, by setting process goals and by focusing on the key mechanisms of the skill under prac- tice. By this way learners become more independent and use strategies facilitating cognitive involvement which are called self-assessment strategies because by using them learners are able to asses their own skill performance (Byra, 2000, 2006). Hence, the first research hypothesis of the study was that the use of “self-check” style of teaching can activate metacognitive processes during physical education and more particularly self-monitoring and planning which are the core components of the self-regulation (Carver & Scheier, 1998). Furthermore, when the teacher assists the students to focus on personal process goals the teacher establishes a task-involving climate and they become more task-involved experiencing higher levels of intrinsic motivation (Digelidis, Papaioannou, Laparidis & Christodoulidis, 2003; Jaakkola & Digelidis, 2007). Conse- quently the second research hypothesis of the study was that the use of “self-check” style of teaching should increase the stu- dents’ levels of intrinsic motivation and that they would perceive Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 111
 A. PAPAIOANNOU ET AL. the climate of their class as more mastery-orientated. These positive motivational and cognitive benefits were not expected if the “practice” style of teaching was used because in this style the physical educator is the only person who is re- sponsible to introduce the skill under learning and its’ key com- ponents, to supervise the students’ progress and to give skill- related feedback providing solutions for the possible incorrect performance. By this way all given information are external, limiting pupils’ opportunities to act autonomously, to become task-involved, to experience high levels of intrinsic motivation and to gain knowledge about how to monitor and to regulate their learning. Indeed, by being less task-involved, students are more likely to observe and compare their peers’ performance with their own. Hence, the “practice” style of teaching was more likely to facilitate performance-approach and performance-avo- idance goals and climates. Method Sample Two hundred and seventy nine pupils (boys = 119, girls = 141, did not provide gender = 19) of seven public elementary schools (13 coeducational classes) participated in the study. The students were in the sixth grade and the classes were taught by seven physical education teachers (years of teaching ex- perience = 15.28 ± 3.09). The study was conducted with the permission of the Ministry of Education and the students agreed to take part voluntarily after the collection of parents’ informed consent. Design and Procedure Prior to the study one of the authors visited the schools which were randomly selected and informed the physical education teachers about the details of the research design. All teachers had complete knowledge and adequate experience on the spec- trums’ styles. Each of them was teaching to two classes of the 6th grade except one teacher who had only one class at the sixth grade. Teachers were asked to teach one of their classes with the “practice” style of teaching and the other with the “self-check” style of teaching. The physical education teacher who had one class taught with the “self-check” style of teaching. In sum, 131 students (n = 52 boys, n = 65 girls, n = 14 did not provide gen- der) were taught with the “self-check” style of teaching and 148 students (n = 67 boys, n = 76 girls, n = 5 did not provide gender) were taught with the “practice” teaching style. The intervention took place during spring and lasted two weeks (2 teaching hours per week, each of them lasting 45 minutes). One of the researchers was present in every lesson to ensure the correct execution of the experimental procedure. Four lesson plans were prepared from the authors and were designed in the same way for all classes (a. warm-up games and activities; b. main-part activities; and c. cool-down games and activities). Warm-up and cool-down games and activities were the same for both self-check and practice groups in every lesson. During the main part of activities of each lesson, one of four football skills (a. dribbling with the upper side of the foot; b. ball passing with inner side of the foot and ball receiving; c. ball shooting with the upper side of foot; and d. throw-in) was in- troduced to pupils of both groups in the same order for each class. The main part of every lesson lasted 20 minutes for all classes and contained two drills/activities. After a short descrip- tion and a demonstration of the skill under learning by physical education teachers, pupils of both groups executed 25 trials of each drill (50 trials). While physical education teachers were responsible to provide feedback to the students of the “practice” group the students of the “self-check” group were provided with skill-related feedback through criterion sheets which were pre- pared by the authors. The criterion sheets included a brief description of each skill’s use, four key-components of it, four pictures illustrating every key-component and a self-check form for the students to mark their success (e.g. dribbling with the upper side of the foot. De- scription: this skill has a main part in ball possession by your team and can be used for surpassing your opponents and for the movement in an open field area. Key components: a. Do I use the upper side of my foot—the place where my shoelaces are? b. Are my legs slightly bended? c. Do I keep the ball in front of my feet? d. Do I keep my head up looking at my part- ners?). Pupils executed ten sets of five trials (50 trials). After each set, for every key-component they indicated in the crite- rion sheet whether they performed the skill correctly () or they needed further improvement (). At the end of the crite- rion card there was a self-assessment form where students could score their success for every set (min score: 0, max score: 4). All participants responded to questionnaires at the end of one lesson before the intervention which was a typical lesson of their class and at the end of the final lesson of the intervention. They were informed that there were no right or wrong answers, were encouraged to answer as honestly as possible, and were assured that their responses would remain confidential. Students worked on their own but those who wanted to ask questions could communicate privately with the researcher who was pre- sent and administered the questionnaires. Measures All students completed the instruments after a 35 minutes class. In order to reduce completion time, shorter versions of the existing questionnaires were administered. Based on data from previous studies with these questionnaires, the best items were selected trying to ensure that these shorter versions would satisfy demands for reliability and factorial validity. As is showed below, we conducted confirmatory factor analyses to establish the factorial validity of the present measures. Moreover, we investigated correlations between the present scales to establish divergent and convergent validity. All correlation results were in line with theoretical assumptions but they are not reported here due to space limitations (however they can be provided by the authors upon request). Achievement Goals The Achievement Goals Scale at the Situational Level of Generality (Papaioannou, Milosis, Kosmidou, & Tsigilis, 2007) was used to assess students’ goal orientations at a particular point of time. This instrument was created to capture changes of students’ achievement goals in physical education classes at a particular point of time and has been used successfully in previous studies (Papaioannou et al., 2007). We removed 9 items from this instrument, the 6 items comprising the social approval factor and one item from the mastery, performance- approach and performance-avoidance factors respectively. Fol- lowing the stem: “In today’s physical education class…” Stu- dents indicated their preferences to the 15 items of this instru- Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 112
 A. PAPAIOANNOU ET AL. ment in a 5-point Likert type scale (5 = strongly agree, … 1 = strongly disagree). Examples of the items are: “… my goal was to improve my skills” (mastery goal, 5 items); “… I was striving in order to be the best” (performance-approach goal, 5 items); “… I was avoiding drills and games for which I may be gibed at for my abilities” (performance-avoidance goal, 5 items). Con- firmatory factor analysis revealed satisfactory goodness-of-fit indices for the three-factor model (chi square = 120.6; df = 87; TLI = .955; CFI = .967; RMSEA = .037). Motivationa l Climate To estimate how students perceive the climate of their physical education classes three scales (mastery climate, performance- approach climate and performance-avoidance climate) of the Perceptions of a Physical Education Teacher’s Emphasis on Achievement Goals Questionnaire at the Situational level of Generality (Papaioannou et al., 2007) were used. For the afore- mentioned reasons and following the same procedure we also reduced the items of this questionnaire to 15 by removing the 7 items of the social approval climate and 1 item from each of the remaining factors. Following the stem: “In today’s physical edu- cation class, our teacher…”, students responded in 5-point Likert type scale (5 = strongly agree, … 1 = strongly disagree). Exam- ple items of this instrument are: “…. was very satisfied when someone was showing improvement after hard effort” (mastery climate, 5 items); “… was pleased with students showing that they were more capable than others” (performance approach climate, 5 items); “… made me worry if they say that I am not capable” (performance avoidance climate, 5 items). The results of the confirmatory analysis confirmed the three-factor struc- ture of the instrument (chi square = 121.89; df = 89; TLI = .941; CFI = .957; RMSEA = .038). Intrinsic Motiva ti o n The two scales (interest/enjoyment and effort/importance) of the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) (McAuley, Duncan & Tammen, 1989) were used to measure students’ intrinsic moti- vation. This questionnaire is one of the most frequently used instrument in the context of sports and physical education and its psychometric properties have been established in previous studies (e.g., Tsigilis & Theodosiou, 2003). Students answered to the items following the stem: “In today’s Physical Education class…” Responses were given on 5-point Likert type scale (5 = strongly agree, … 1 = strongly disagree). Example items are: “… the activities were fun to do” (interest/enjoyment, 4 items); “… I was trying very hard” (effort/importance, 4 items). The present confirmatory analysis revealed satisfactory goodness- of-fit indices for the two-factor model (chi square = 35.5; df = 19; TLI = .961; CFI = .979; RMSEA = .056). Metacognitive Self-Regulation Two scales measuring planning and self- monitoring from the Metacognitive Processes in Physical Education Question- naire (MPIPEQ) were administered. This instrument is an en- riched and adapted version for physical education of the Meta- cognitive Awareness Inventory (MAI) (Schraw & Dennison, 1994), which is based on Brown’s (1987) framework. Its’ con- struct validity and reliability has been tested in previous studies (Theodosiou & Papaioannou, 2006; Theodosiou, Papaioannou, & Mantis, 2005; Theodosiou, Mantis, & Papaioannou, 2008). We were particularly interested in measuring “self-monitoring” and “planning” because these cognitive functions were ex- pected to be activated through the use of the “self-check” style. All items were adjusted at the situational level of generality. Following the stem “In today’s Physical Education class…” responses were given on 5-point Likert scales (5 = always, 4 = often, 3 = sometimes, 2 = rarely, 1 = never). Example items of the two scales are: “… it was clear to me what I wanted to learn” (planning, 4 items); “… as I was learning new exercises I was checking if I was actually doing well” (self-monitoring, 4 items). Confirmatory analysis revealed excellent goodness-of- fit indices for the present two-factor model (chi square = 22.6; df = 19; TLI = .990; CFI = .995; RMSEA = .026). From the remaining 7 scales of the MPIPEQ we selected one item from every scale in order to capture overall metacognitive activity in physical education. These items were as follows: declarative knowledge: “… I realized which exercises I could perform right”, conditional knowledge: “… when I wanted to grow better in a game I put into practice a learning strategy”, procedural knowledge: “… I had a clear view of how to put in practice a learning method I have been taught”, imagery: “… before I perform an exercise I imagined myself to perform it”, information management: “… I thought if the games I played were similar to others”, problem solving strategies: “… when I got confused I stopped to see the whole thing from the begin- ning”, evaluation: “… since I have learned an exercise I com- pared the way I had learned it with other ways”. The good- ness-of-fit indices for this one-factor model were satisfactory (chi square = 31.4; df =14; TLI = .920; CFI = .947; RMSEA = .067). Results Internal Consi ste ncie s an d Scale Construction The alpha reliabilities (Cronbach, 1951) of the scales of the questionnaires for both measurements are shown in Table 1. Most of the results indicate acceptable scale reliabilities. Scale scores were computed for measurements before and after the intervention. Descriptive statistics for these scales using each individual student as the unit of analysis are shown in Table 2. Based on Silverman’s and Skonie’s (1997) recommendation that experi- mental effects in school settings should be also studied at a class level, scale scores were also computed using class as the unit of analysis. In order to have enough sample size of classes in sub- sequent analyses, given the sufficient number of boys and girls Table 1. Internal consistency of the scales. Initial measure Final measure Mastery goal .73 .71 Performance-approach goal .78 .78 Performance-avoidance goal .78 .81 Mastery climate .74 .71 Performance-approach climate.72 .55 Performance-avoidance climate.72 .67 Enjoyment .69 .81 Effort .73 .79 Self-monitoring .75 .75 Planning .66 .79 Overall metacognitive activity .66 .79 Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 113
 A. PAPAIOANNOU ET AL. Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 114 Table 2. Means and standard deviations for goal orientations, motivational climate, intrinsic motivation and metacognitive self-regulation. Unit of analysis: student Unit of analysis: class Measure: Initial Final Initial Final Teaching style: Self-check Practice Self-check Practice Self-check Practice Self-check Practice M SD M SDM SDM SDM SDM SD M SD M SD Mastery goal 4.34a.66 4.16b .66 4.32a.56 3.85c.75 4.23d.33 4.23d.33 4.32d .18 3.78f.33 Performance-approach goal 2.97a.99 2.82a 1.02 2.18b.65 2.66c1.03 2.99d.53 2.82d.40 2.21e .28 2.70d.49 Performance-avoidance goal 2.69a1.01 2.81a .97 2.35b.75 2.75a.92 2.64d.34 2.81d.27 2.37e .30 2.74d.39 Mastery climate 3.95a.74 3.88a .72 4.27b.60 3.65c.76 3.93d.33 3.80d.41 4.25e .21 3.60f.30 Performance-approach climate 2.74a.72 2.65a .77 2.33b.64 2.74a.84 2.63de .36 2.67df .35 2.35e .18 2.76f.37 Performance-avoidance climate 2.71a.85 2.58a .77 2.26b.69 2.75a.86 2.69d.35 2.55d.31 2.27e .24 2.74d.41 Enjoyment 4.29a.67 4.32a .70 4.51b.52 4.07c.89 4.27d.254.32d.39 4.50d .20 3.98e.44 Effort 4.03a.97 4.05a .80 4.37b.60 3.74c.994.06d.29 4.03d.29 4.40e .20 4.03f.29 Self-monitoring 3.89a.85 3.75a .93 4.25b.67 3.50c.86 3.90d.31 3.73d.42 4.27e .32 3.46f.42 Planning 3.87a.85 3.69a .88 4.20b.63 3.53a1.02 3.88d.32 3.69d.42 4.21e .27 3.50f.35 Overall metacognitive activity 3.49a.70 3.42a .63 4.20b.55 3.36a.68 3.51d.22 3.43d.24 4.22e .23 3.36d.20 Note: Group means sharing the same subscript are not significantly different at the .05 level. in each class (for each gender in every class: 15 > n > 6), scale scores for each class were computed separately for each gender. Descriptive statistics for scale scores using class as the unit of analysis are shown in Table 2. Differences between “Self-Check” and “Practice” Style of Teaching To investigate possible differences between “self-check” and “practice” styles in goal orientations, motivational climates, in- trinsic motivation, self-monitoring, planning and overall meta- cognitive activity, four doubly repeated measures MANOVAs were computed using student as the unit of analysis. In each of these MANOVAs, independent variable was the teaching style and depended variables were the following variables before and after the intervention: 1) The three goal orientations; 2) The three climate scales; 3) The two intrinsic motivation variables; and 4) The three metacognitive variables. Results from all doubly re- peated measures MANOVAs revealed significant interaction ef- fects of the repeated measures factor with the teaching styles factor (p < .001). To investigate these effects for each dependent variable we conducted repeated measures ANOVAs (measurement × teach- ing style). The results revealed significant interactions between measurement and teaching style in all depended variables (Ta- ble 3; Figures 1-11). In order to better understand these inter- actions separate simple effect analyses were performed (Table 2). From the initial to the final measurement, in comparison to the scores of students who were taught with the “self-check” style of teaching, the scores of students who were taught with the “practice” style were decreased for scales capturing mastery goal, mastery climate, intrinsic motivation (enjoyment and effort), overall metacognitive activity, self-monitoring and planning (Ta- ble 2, Figures 1, 3, 7-9 and 11) but they were increased for scales measuring performance goals and performance-oriented climates. The same statistical analyses were contacted using class level as the unit of analyses. Almost similar results emerged, con- firming most of our research hypotheses. Significant interact- tions (p < .001) emerged between the repeated measures factor and the teaching styles factor in all doubly repeated measures MANOVAs. As it can be seen in Table 3, results from repeated measures ANOVAs (measurement × teaching style) for each dependent variable revealed significant interactions between measurement and teaching style in almost all the depended variables. Com- pared to classes that were taught with “self-check” style of teach- ing, classes that were taught with “practice” style of teaching had lower scores in the final measurement in scales measuring mastery goal, mastery-oriented climate, intrinsic motivation and metacognitive activity and higher scores in scales measuring performance goals and performance-oriented climates. Discussion It has been suggested that the “self-check” style of teaching assists students to set process goals (Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 1996; Papaioannou & Kouli, 1999). The present study tried to elaborate deeper into this matter by exploring the cognitive processes that are activated when students are taught with the “self-check” style of teaching. The aim of this study was to verify the notion that teaching physical education skills with the “self-check teaching style” has important benefits for students’ motivation and self-regulation. Specifically, it was hypothesized that the adoption of this style of teaching has positive effects on motivational climate and students’ goal orientations, intrinsic motivation and metacogni- tive activity in physical education classes. All hypotheses were confirmed. The results showed that the “self-check” style helps students to activate two core processes of the self-regulation system, namely, self-monitoring and planning. Through the use of the criteria sheet, pupils are asked to monitor themselves, to ana- lyse their performance and to decide about the correctness of their actions. Then, by applying this information they are asked to set new or to re-establish process goals for the improvement of their motor performance in the next trials. Through this pro- cedure students complete the three phases of self-regulated learn- ing (forethought, performance, self-reflection; Zimmerman 1986; 2000) and adjust their actions by themselves using personal feedback loops which involve the activation of conscious cog- nitive processes. Such feedback loops constitute the basis of self-regulation (Carver & Scheier, 1998) and the activation of them requires metacognitive knowledge and metacognitive regu-
 A. PAPAIOANNOU ET AL. Table 3. Results of the repeated measures ANOVAs (measurement × teaching style). Unit of analysis: student Unit of analysis: class F Sig. η2 F Sig. η2 Mastery goal 10.76 .001 .035 14.37 .001 .374 Performance-approach goal 23.53 .000 .079 14.04 .001 .369 Performance-avoidance goal 4.98 .026 .018 1.39 .249 .055 Mastery climate 28.86 .000 .095 16.32 .000 .405 Performance-approach climate 23.15 .000 .077 3.66 .068 .132 Performance-avoidance climate 30.01 .000 .098 12.71 .002 .346 Enjoyment 20.52 .000 .069 10.96 .003 .314 Effort 29.53 .000 .097 46.28 .000 .658 Self-monitoring 27.64 .000 .091 25.72 .000 .517 Planning 15.31 .000 .053 22.42 .000 .483 Overall metacognitive activity 69.79 .000 .202 96.48 .000 .801 ,5 3 ,5 4 ,5 Initial measur.Final measur. self-check practise 4.5 4 3.5 3 2.5 Figure 1. Changes in mastery goal orientation. ,5 2 ,5 3 Initial measur.Final measur. self-check practise 3 2.5 2 1.5 Figure 2. Changes in performance approach goal orientation. 1,5 2 ,5 3 ,5 Initial measur.Final measur. self-check practise 3.5 3 2.5 2 1.5 Figure 3. Changes in performance avoidance goal orientation. ,5 3 ,5 4 ,5 Initial measur.Final measur. self-check practise 4.5 4 3.5 3 2.5 Figure 4. Changes in mastery motivational climate. 1 ,5 2 ,5 3 Initial measur.Final measur. self-check practise 3 2.5 2 1.5 1 Figure 5. Changes in performance approach motivational climate. ,5 2 ,5 3 ,5 Initial measur.Final measur. self-check practise 3.5 3 2.5 2 1.5 Figure 6. Changes in performance avoidance motivational climate. 3 ,5 4 ,5 5 Initial measur.Final measur. self-check practise 5 4.5 4 3.5 3 Figure 7. Changes in enjoyment in physical education. ,5 3 ,5 4 ,5 Initial measur.Final measur. self-check practise 4.5 4 3.5 3 2.5 Figure 8. Changes in effort in physical education. ,5 3 ,5 4 ,5 self-check practise 4.5 4 3.5 3 2.5 Figure 9. Changes in self-monitoring. ,5 3 ,5 4 ,5 Initial measur.Final measur. self-check practise 4.5 4 3.5 3 2.5 Figure 10. Changes in planning. Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 115
 A. PAPAIOANNOU ET AL. ,5 3 ,5 4 , Initial measur.Final measur. self-check practise 4.5 4 3.5 3 2.5 Figure 11. Changes in general metacognitive activity. lation. Indeed, the present findings provided strong support to this notion by revealing large effect sizes. Students who were taught the soccer skills in the “self-check” conditions were much more likely to use several components of metacognitive knowl- edge and regulation (i.e., overall metacognitive activity) than students who were taught with the practice style. Apart from the cognitive benefits, the use of the “self-check” style has motivational benefits too. This study confirmed that the “self-check” style contributes to the development of a posi- tive motivational climate which is characterized as high mas- tery and low performance oriented. Specifically, by setting proc- ess goals students individualize the learning process, which is an important dimension of a mastery-oriented climate (Ames, 1992). Indeed, the present findings confirmed that the “self-check” style of teaching had more positive effects on perceptions of mastery-oriented climate than the practice style. This individu- alized learning process helped students to sustain their attention on their learning task and to be less distracted by others’ be- haviours and performance in comparison to students who were taught with the “practice” style of teaching. In sum, this led to decreased perceptions of social comparison and consequently reduced perceptions of teachers’ emphasis on performance ap- proach or performance avoidance goals in the “self-check” style classes. The positive effects of the “self-check” style on motivational climate led to positive consequences in students’ adoption of mastery and performance goals. Students in “self-check” classes adopted higher mastery goals and lower performance goals than students in “practice style” classes. By being more self-regulated, students in “self-check” classes were also more autonomous learners than students in “practice style” classes. By attending less the performance of others, students’ progress in “self-check” conditions was more self-determined in comparison to students in “practice style” conditions. These higher levels of autonomy and self-determination led to higher intrinsic motivation (Deci & Ryan, 1985) for students in the “self-check” conditions in comparison to students in the “practise style” conditions. In total, in comparison to preferable teacher-centred styles such as the “practice” style (Curtner-Smith, Todorovich, McCau- ghtry, & Lacon, 2001), the present results were clearly in fa- vour of the “self-check” style. It appears that this style of teach- ing establishes an educational environment which includes af- fordances for self-regulation. These findings refer to begin- ners, but further research should examine whether these impli- cations can be generalized across students with advanced know- ledge and skills because in the opinion of several researchers there are many types of self-regulated action that are more or less suitable for different tasks, in different domains, and for different students. On the subject of metacognitive regulation the present study clearly focused on self-monitoring and plan- ning. Thus, longer interventions involving a number of combi- nations between “self-check” and other styles of teaching are needed, because they better reflect the multidimensionality of the teaching process than the limited number of the present classes. Implications for Physical Education Teachers The present findings imply that in grades 5 - 8 which is a pe- riod when students are introduced to new sports skills, the use of the “self-check” style could have a prominent role in the teaching process. However, this conclusion has not to be con- sidered as an effort to verify the supremacy of one style over another. Mosston and Ashworth (1986) emphasize that no sin- gle style of teaching is superior to another and suggest the physi- cal education teacher to determine first what has to be accom- plished and then to decide which style is more appropriate in a given situation. For example, early research findings (Beckett, 1990) confirmed that the “practise” is equivalent to the “inclu- sion” teaching style in cases of performing a motor skill. But between these two styles the “inclusion” style of teaching is preferable when the students are girls and when the enhance- ment of perceived athletic competence is the goal of the lesson, (Chatoupis & Emmanouel, 2003). Thus, the main message of this study is that teachers concerning in promoting self-regulatory skills and in establishing a mastery-oriented climate in physical education classes can choose “self-check” style to achieve their goals. Having in mind the above, the adequate training of the physical education teachers about the implementation and the precise usefulness of each style becomes a main priority. Physi- cal education authorities through seminars and workshops have to ensure that physical education teachers have adequate knowl- edge of the variety of teaching styles in order to choose which one better achieve their goals. REFERENCES Abd Al-Salam, A. (2004). The effects of three styles of teaching on the performance level and practice trials of long serves and short serves in badminton. Dirasat: Educationa l Sciences, 31, 88-104. AlMulla-Abdullah, F. (2003). The effectiveness of the reciprocal teach- ing style on the level of shooting skills acquisition in team handball. Journal of Educational Psychological Science, 3, 10-37. Ames, C. (1984). Competitive, cooperative and individualistic goal structures: A motivational analysis. In R. Ames, & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in education. Vol. 1: Student motivation (pp. 177-207). New York: Academic Press. Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261-271. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.84.3.261 Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1987). Mothers’ belief about the role of ability and effort in school learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 18, 409-414. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.79.4.409 Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: Students’ learning strategies and motivation processes. Journal of Educational Psycholo g y , 80, 260-267. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.80.3.260 Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundation of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-Efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman. Beckett, K. (1990). The effects of two teaching styles on college stu- dents’ achievement of selected physical education outcomes. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 10, 153-169. Boekaerts, M. (1997). Self-regulated learning: A new concept embraced by researchers, policy makers, educators, teachers and students. Learn- ing and Instruction, 7, 161-186. doi:10.1016/S0959-4752(96)00015-1 Boyce, B. A. (1992). The effects of three styles of teaching on univer- Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 116
 A. PAPAIOANNOU ET AL. sity student’s motor performance. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 11, 389-401. Brown, A. (1987). Metacognition, executive control, self-regulation and other more mysterious mechanisms. In F. Weinert, & R. Kluwer (Eds.), Metacognition, motivation and understanding (pp. 65-116). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Byra, M. (2000). A review of spectrum research: The contribution of two eras. Quest, 52, 229-245. doi:10.1080/00336297.2000.10491712 Byra, M. (2006). Teaching styles and inclusive pedagogies. In D. Kirk, D. MacDonald, & M. O’Sullivan (Eds.), The handbook of physical education (pp. 449-466). London: Sage Publications. Carr, S., & Weigand, D. A. (2002). The influence of significant others on the goal orientations of youngsters in physical education. Journal of Sport Behavior, 25, 19-40. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). Self-regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge university press. doi:10.1017/CBO9781139174794 Chen, D., & Singer, R. N. (1992). Self-regulation and cognitive strate- gies in sport participation. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 23, 277-300. Crews, D. J. (1993). Self-regulation strategies in sport and exercise. In R. N. Singer, M. Murphey, & L. K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of re- search on sport psychology (pp. 557-568). New York: Macmillan Publishing Company. Cronbach, L. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of test. Psychometrika, 16, 297-334. doi:10.1007/BF0230555 Cross, D. R., & Paris, S. G. (1988). Developmental an instructional analy- sis of children’s metacognition and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 8 0, 131-142. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.80.2.131 Cunningham, G. B., & Xiang, P. (2008). Testing the mediating role of motivational climate in the relationship between achievement goals and satisfaction: Are the relationships invariant across sex? Journal of Teaching in Physical Ed ucation, 27, 192-204. Curtner-Smith, M. D., Todorovich, J. R., McCaughtry, N. A., & Lacon, S. A. (2001). Urban teachers’ use of productive and reproductive teaching styles within the confines of the National Curriculum for physical education. European Physical Education Review, 7, 177- 190. doi:10.1177/1356336X010072005 Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-deter- mination in human beha v i o r . NY: Plenum. Digelidis, N., Papaioannou, A., Laparidis, K., & Christodoulidis, T. (2003). A one-year intervention in 7th grade physical education classes aiming to change motivational climate and attitudes towards exercise. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 4, 195-210. doi:10.1016/S1469-0292(02)00002-X Duda, J. L., Cumming, J., & Balaguer, I. (2005). Enhancing athletes’ self regulation, task involvement, and self determination via psycho- logical skills training. In D. Hackfort, J. Duda, & R. Lidor (Eds.), Handbook of applied sport psychology research: International per- spectives (pp. 143-165). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Tech- nology. Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256-273. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.256 Efklides, A. (2001). Metacognitive experiences in problem solving: Me- tacognition, motivation, and self-regulation. In A. Efklides, J. Kuhl, & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Trends and prospects in motivation re- search (pp. 297-323). Dordrecht: Kluwer. Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and achieve- ment goals. Edu cational P sychologist, 34, 169-189. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3403_3 Epstein, J. L. (1989). Family structures and student motivation: A de- velopmental perspective. In C. Ames, & R. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in education: Vol. 3. Goals and cognitions. Orlando: Academic Press. Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906-911. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.34.10.906 Chatoupis, C., & Emmanuel, C. (2003). “The effects of two disparate instructional approaches on students” self-perceptions in elementary physical education. European Journal o f Sp o r t S c i e nc e , 3, 1-16. doi:10.1080/17461390300073101 Ford, J. K., Smith, E. M., Weissbein, D. A., Gully, S. M., & Salas, E. (1998). Relationships of goal orientation, metacognitive activity and practice strategies with learning outcomes and transfer. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 218-233. doi:10.1037//0021-9010.83.2.218 Gano-Overway, L. A. (2008). The effect of goal involvement on self- regulatory processes. International Journal of Sports and Exercise Psychology, 6, 132-156. doi:10.1080/1612197X.2008.9671858 Goldberger, M., & Gerney, P. (1986). The effects of direct teaching styles on motor skill acquisition of fifth grade children. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 57, 215-219. Goldberger, M., Gerney, P., & Chamberlain J. (1982). The effects of three styles of teaching on the psychomotor performance of fifth grade children. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 53, 116- 124. Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (1994). Role of parental motivational practices in children’s academic intrinsic mo- tivation and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 104-113. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.86.1.104 Goudas, M., Biddle, S., Fox, K., & Underwood, M. (1995). It ain’t what you do, it’s the way that you do it! Teaching style affects chil- dren’s motivation in track and field. The Sport Psychologist, 18, 462-471. Guan, J., Xiang, P., McBride, R. E., & Bruene, A. (2006). Achievement goals, social goals, and students’ reported persistence and effort in high school physical education. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 25, 58-74. Hidi, S, & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Motivating the academically unmotivated: A critical issue for the 21st century. Review of Educa- tional Research, 70, 151-179. doi:10.2307/1170660 Jaakkola, T. & Digelidis, N. (2007). Establishing a positive motiva- tional climate in physical education. In J. Liukkonen, Y. Vanden Au- weele, B. Vereijken, D. Alfermann, & Y. Theodorakis (Eds.), Psy- chology for physical educators: Student in focus (2nd ed.; pp. 3-20). Champaign: Human Kinetics. Jacobs, J., & Paris, S. (1987). Children’s metacognition about reading. Issues in definition, measurement, and instruction. Educational Psy- chologist, 22, 255-278. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep2203&4_4 Kirschenbaum, D. S. (1984). Self-regulation and sport psychology: Nurturing an emerging symbiosis. Journal of Sport Psychology, 6, 159-183. Kirschenbaum, D. S. (1987). Self-regulation and sport performance. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exerc ise, 19, S106-S113. doi:10.1249/00005768-198710001-00003 Kirschenbaum, D. S., & Wittrock, D. A. (1984). Cognitive-behavioral interventions in sport: A self-regulatory perspective. In J. M. Silva, & R. S. Weinberg (Eds.), Psychological foundations of sport (pp. 81-90). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Loyens, S. M. M., Joshua, M., & Rikers, R. M. J. P. (2008). Self-di- rected learning in problem-based learning and its relationships with self-regulated learning. Educational Psychological Review, 20, 411- 427. doi:10.1007/s10648-008-9082-7 Marshall, H. H., & Weinstein. R. S. (1986). Classroom context of stu- dent perceived differential teacher treatment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 441-453. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.78.6.441 McAuley, E., Duncan, T., & Tammen, V. (1989). Psychometric proper- ties of the intrinsic motivation inventory in a competitive sport set- ting: A confirmatory factor analysis. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 60, 48-58. McCarthy, P. J., Jones, M. V., & Clark-Carter, D. (2008). Understand- ing enjoyment in youth sport: A developmental perspective. Psy- chology of Sport and Exercise, 9, 142-156. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2007.01.005 Morgan, K., Kingston, K., & Sproule, J. (2005). Effects of different teaching styles on the teacher behaviours that influence motivational climate and pupils’ motivation in physical education. European Physical Education Revie w , 1 1 , 257-285. doi:10.1177/1356336X05056651 Mosston, M. (1966). Teaching physical education. Columbus, OH: Merrill. Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 117
 A. PAPAIOANNOU ET AL. Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 118 Mosston, M., & Ashworth, S. (1994). Teaching physical education. Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill. Mosston, M., & Ashworth, S. (1986). Teaching physical education (4th ed.). New York: Mcmillan. Mosston, M., & Ashworth, S. (2002). Teaching physical education (5th ed.). Boston: Benjamin Cummings. Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance. Psychological Review, 91, 328-346. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.91.3.328 Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Ommundsen, Y. (2003). Implicit theories of ability and self-regulation strategies in physical education classes. Educational Psychology, 23, 141-157. doi:10.1080/01443410303224 Ommundsen, Y. (2006). Pupils’ self-regulation in physical education: The role of motivational climates and differential achievement goals. European Physical Education Review, 12, 289-315. doi:10.1177/1356336X06069275 Otero, J., & Campanario, J. (1992). The relationship between academic achievement and metacognitive comprehension monitoring ability of Spanish secondary school students. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52, 419-430. doi:10.1177/0013164492052002017 Papaioannou, A., & Kouli, O. (1999). The effect of task structure, per- ceived motivational climate and goal orientations on students’ task involvement and anxiety. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 11, 51-71. doi:10.1080/10413209908402950 Papaioannou, A., Ampatzoglou, G., Kalogiannis, P., & Sagovits, A. (2008). Social agents, achievement goals, satisfaction and academic achievement in youth sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 9, 122-141. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2007.02.003 Papaioannou, A., Milosis, D., Kosmidou, E., & Tsigilis, N. (2007). Motivational climate and achievement goals at the situational level of generality. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 19, 38-66. doi:10.1080/10413200601113778 Papaioannou, A., Simou, T., Kosmidou, E., Milosis, D., & Tsigilis, N. (2009). Goal orientations at the global level of generality and in physical education: Their association with self-regulation, affect, be- liefs and behaviours. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, 466-480. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2009.01.003 Parsons, J. E., Adler, T. F., & Kaczala, C. M. (1982). Socialization of achievement attitudes and beliefs: Parental influences. Child Devel- opment, 53, 310-321. doi:10.2307/1128973 Petlichkoff, L. M. (2003). Self-regulation skills for children and ado- lescents. In M. R. Weiss (Ed.), Developmental sport and exercise psychology: A lifespan perspective (pp. 269-288). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology. Pintrich, P. R. (2000). Multiple goals, multiple pathways: The role of goal orientation in learning and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 544-555. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.92.3.544 Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regu- lated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33-40. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.33 Pintrich, P. R., & Garcia, T. (1991). Student goal orientation and self- regulation in the college classroom. In Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 7). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Schraw, G., & Dennison, R. S. (1994). Assessing metacognitive aware- ness. Contemporary Edu c a t i o n al Psychology, 19, 460-475. doi:10.1006/ceps.1 994.1033 Silverman, S., & Skonie, R. (1997). Research on teaching in physical education: An analysis of published research. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 16, 300-311. Solmon, M. A., & Boone, J. (1993). The impact of student goal orienta- tion in physical education classes. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 64, 418-424. Solmon, M. A., & Lee, A. M. (1997). Development of an instrument to assess cognitive processes in physical education classes. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 68, 152-160. Stipek, D., & Hoffman, J. (1980). Children’s achievement-related ex- pectancies as a function of academic performance histories and sex. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 861-865. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.72.6.861 Theodosiou, A. (2004). Metacognitive strategies and motivation cli- mate in physical education. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Ko- motini: Democritus University of Thrace. Theodosiou, A., & Papaioannou, A. (2006). Motivational climate, achieve- ment goals and metacognitive activity in physical education and ex- ercise involvement in out-of-school settings. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7, 361-379. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2005.10.002 Theodosiou, A., Mantis, K., & Papaioannou, A. (2008). Student self- reports of metacognitive activity in physical education classes. Age- group differences and the effect of goal orientations and perceived motivational climate. Educational Research and Reviews, 3, 353-364. Theodosiou, A., Papaioannou, A., & Mantis, K. (2005). Factor structure and discriminant validity of the metacognitive processes in physical education questionnaire. Scientific Annuals of Psychological Society of Northern Greece, 3, 91-118. Thill, E., & Brunel, P. (1995). Ego involvement and task involvement: Related conceptions of ability, effort, and learning strategies among soccer players. International Jo u rn a l of Sport Psychology, 26, 81-97. Tsigilis, N., & Theodosiou, A. (2003). Temporal stability of the intrin- sic motivation inventory. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 97, 271-280. doi:102466/PMS.97.4.271-280 Veenman, M. V. J., Van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M., & Afflerbach, P. (2006). Metacognition and learning: Conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacognition an d L earning, 1, 3-14. doi:10.1007/s11409-006-6893-0 Viciana, J., Cervello, E. M., & Ramirez-Lechga, J. (2007). Effect of manipulating positive and negative feedback on goal orientations, perceived motivational climate, satisfaction, task choice, perception of ability, and attitude toward physical education lessons. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 105, 67-82. doi:10.2466/PMS.105.5.67-82 Vrugt, A., & Oort, F. J. (2008). Metacognition, achievement goals, study strategies and academic achievement: Pathways to achieve- ment. Metacognition and Learning, 30, 123-146. doi:10.1007/s11409-008-9022-4 Weinstein, R. S., & Middlestadt, S. E. (1979). Student perception of teacher interactions with male high and low achievers. Journal of Educational Psycholo g y , 71, 421-431. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.71.4.421 Xiang, P., McBride, R., & Bruene, A. (2003). Relations of parents’ beliefs to children’s motivation in an elementary physical education running program. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 22, 410-425. Zimmerman, B. J. (1986). Becoming a self-regulated learner: Which are the key subprocesses? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 11, 307-313. doi:10.1016/0361-476X(86)90027-5 Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: A social cognitive perspective. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 13-39). New York: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/B978-012109890-2/50031-7 Zimmerman, B. J. & Kitsantas, A. (1996). Self-regulated learning of a motoric skill: The role of goal setting and self-monitoring. Journal of Applied Sports Psychology, 8, 60-75. doi:10.1080/10413209608406308 Zimmerman, B. J., & Kitsantas, A. (1997). Developmental phases in self-regulation: Shifting from process goals to outcome goals. Jour- nal of Educational Psychology, 89, 29-36. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.89.1.29
|