Open Journal of Psychiatry, 2011, 1, 106-109
doi:10.4236/ojpsych.2011.13015 Published Online October 2011 (http://www.SciRP.org/journal/OJPsych/
OJPsych
).
Published Online October 2011 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/OJPsych
Relationships between cognitive emotion regulation strategies
with depression and anxiety
Majid Pourfaraj Omran
Islamic Azad University, Behshahr Branch, Behshahr, Iran.
Email: m.pourfaraj@gmail.com
Received 20 August 2011; revised 16 September 2011; accepted 10 October 2011.
ABSTRACT
In current study firstly, psychometric properties of
Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ)
in an Iranian sample were examined. The result of
factor analysis, subscales correlations, internal con-
sistency and test-retest coefficients showed good psy-
chometric properties of CERQ in Iran. Secondly, rela -
tionships between cognitive emotion regulation stra-
tegies with depression and anxiety were studied by
multiple regression analysis. The result showed that
catastrophizing, self-blame and rumination were re-
lated with high level of anxiety and depression and
refocusing, positive r e appraisal and planning subscales
rel ated with low level of anxiety and depression.
Keywords: Cognitive Emotion Regulation; Depression;
Anxiety
1. INTRODUCTION
Emotion regulation includes a wide range of conscious
and unconscious physiological, behavioral, and cogni-
tive strategies that are used to reduce, maintain, or in-
crease an emotion [1]. Cognitive emotion regulation
(CER) involves the cognitive way of handling the intake
of emotionally arousing information [2,3]. A relatively
new scale, the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Question-
naire (CERQ), was developed to assess cognitive coping
to stressful life events. This questionnaire includes nine
cognitive strategies: self-blame, referring to thoughts of
putting the blame of what person has experienced on
her/himself; other-blame, referring to thoughts of putting
the blame of what person has experienced on the envi-
ronment or another person; rumination, referring to
thinking about the feelings and thoughts associated with
the negative event; catastrophizing, referring to thoughts
of explicitly emphasizing the terror of what person has
experienced; putting into Persp ective, referring to down-
grading the importance of the event; positive refocusing,
referring to thinking about positive experiences instead
of thinking about the actual event; positive reappraisal,
referring to thoughts of giving the event a positive mean-
ing in terms of personal growth; acceptance, referring to
thoughts of resigning person to what has happened and
planning, referring to thinking about what steps to take
and how to handle the negative event [4].
The CERQ has been included in an increasing number
of studies in various countries focusing on relationships
between cognitive processes and mental health. Despite
some variation across studies, cognitive strategies such as
self-blame, rumination, catastrophizing and positive reap-
praisal (inversely) have consistently been associated
with negative emotions like depression, anxiety, stress
and anger [4-9].
In this research for obtain more knowledge about cross
cultural difference about CER and its role in anxiety and
depression, firstly, psychometric properties of CERQ in
an Iranian sample was studied and then relationship be-
tween CER with anxiety and depression was examined.
2. METHOD
2.1. Subjects
Four hundred eighty four undergraduate students (265 ma-
les) with randomize cluster sampling were selected from
various departments of Shiraz University. The age of the
samples ranged from 18 to 32 years with a mean of 22.14
(SD = 4.02).
2.2. Instruments
2.2.1. Cogni tive Emotion Re gul a ti o n Q uestionnaire
(CERQ)
The CERQ was developed in 1999 for measuring the cog-
nitive emotion regulation strategies that subjects use in
response to the experience of stressful life events [6]. The
CERQ is a self-report questionnaire and has 36-item and 9
subscales (i.e., self-blame, other-blame, rumination, cata-
strophizing, putting into perspective, positive refocusing,
positive reappraisal, acceptance and planning). Items are
M. P. Omran / Open Journal of Psychiatry 1 (2011) 106-109 107
measured on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (al-
most never) to 5 (almost always). The CERQ can be use-
d to measure cognitive strategies that characterize the in-
dividual’s style of responding to stressful events as well as
cognitive strategies that are used in a particular stressful
event or situation, depending on the nature of the ques-
tions under study. In this study was used from questions
about stressful events. Individual subscale scores are ob-
tained by summing up the scores belonging to the par-
ticular subscale (ranging from 4 to 20). The psychometric
properties of the CERQ have been proven to be good
[10,11], with Cronbach’s alpha coefficients in most cases
well over 0.70 and in many cases even over 0.80. Moreover,
the CERQ has been shown to have good factorial validity,
discriminative properties and construct validity [6].
2.2.2. Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II)
The BDI-II, second edition of BDI, is a 21-item measure
that is widely used to assess cognitive, affective, somatic,
and behavioral symptoms of depression. Scores on the
BDI have range from 0 (no symptoms) to 63 (very se-
vere symptoms). The psychometric properties of the
scale are supported [12]. In Iran the psychometric prop-
erties of a Persian-language version of the this inventory
examined with compared mean item scores on the BDI-
II-Persian with those on the English-language version
administered to North American college students [13]. In
this study assessed internal consistency and test- retest reli-
ability and its concurrent validity against a measure of
negative automatic thoughts in depression, the Automa-
tic Thoughts Questionnaire [14]. They also examined the
factor structure of the BDI-II-Persian through comparing
the fit of various proposed models to the data using con-
firmatory factor analysis. The BDI-II-Persian had high
internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87) and ac-
ceptable test-retest reliability (r = 0.74). The instrument
correlated strongly with the Automatic Thoughts Ques-
tionnaire. In factor analysis, models with strongly corre-
lated affective-cognitive and somatic-vegetative factors
provided a better fit than models with one global factor.
2.2.3. State-Trait A nxiety In ventory (STAI)
The STAI is a 40-item self-report measure of general an-
xiety. The first 20 items assess state anxiety, or how the
participant feels ‘right now;’ the second 20 items assess
trait anxiety, or how the participant feels ‘generally.’ The
STAI has high reliability and validity [15]. Mahram [16]
in his study about standardization STAI in Iran reported
this inventory has good psychometric properties in Iran.
2.3. Procedure
The instruments were administered during or after regular
class hours. Before the administration, instructions about
procedure of responding to inventories were given to the
participants.
3. RESULT
3.1. Factor Structure
The factorial structure of the Persian version of the CERQ
was investigated with an exploratory principal components
(PCA) followed by varimax rotation. Items were in-
cluded on a factor if the loading was 0.40 or higher. This
analysis revealed nine factors with eigenvalues greater
than 1, accounting for 76% of the total variance. The
results of factor analyses were summarized in Table 1.
Table 1. Factor structure of CERQ.
Factors and items Loading
Self-blame
I feel that I am the one to blame for it 0.68
I feel that I am the one who is responsible for what has
happened 0.71
I think about the mistakes I have made in this matter 0.64
I think that basically the cause must lie within myself 0.75
Other-blame
I feel that others are to blame for it 0.72
I feel that others are responsible for what has happened 0.64
I think about the mistakes others have made in this matter 0.59
I feel that basically the cause lies with others 0.61
Rumination
I often think about how I feel about what I have experi-
enced 0.69
I am preoccupied with what I think and feel about what I
have experienced 0.78
I want to understand why I feel the way I do about what I
have experienced 0.66
I dwell upon the feelings the situation has evoked in me 0.63
Catastrophizing
I often think that what I have experienced is much worse
than what others have experienced 0.77
I keep thinking about how terrible it is what I have experi-
enced 0.71
I often think that what I have experienced is the worst tha
t
can happen to a person 0.72
I continually think how horrible the situation has been 0.75
Refocus on planning
I think of what I can do best 0.69
I think about how I can best cope with the situation 0.73
I think about how to change the situation 0.65
I think about a plan of what I can do best 0.79
Positive reappraisal
I think I can learn something from the situation 0.58
I think that I can become a stronger person as a result o
f
what has happened 0.66
I think that the situation also has its positive sides 0.71
I look for the positive sides to the matter 0.75
Putting into perspective
I think that it all could have been much worse 0.56
I think that other people go through much worse experi-
ences 0.69
I think that it hasn’t been too bad compared to other things 0.71
I tell myself that there are worse things in life 0.67
Acceptance
I think that I have to accept that this has happened 0.70
I think that I have to accept the situation 0.64
I think that I cannot change anything about it 0.71
I think that I must learn to live with it 0.57
Positive refocusing
I think of nicer things than what I have experienced 0.70
I think of pleasant things that have nothing to do with it 0.68
I think of something nice instead of what has happened 0.59
I think about pleasant experiences 0.65
C
opyright © 2011 SciRes. OJPsych
M. P. Omran / Open Journal of Psychiatry 1 (2011) 106-109
Copyright © 2011 SciRes.
108
3.2. Reliability
Internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha), and test-retest
coefficients for with 3 weeks interval (N = 30; one class
of original sample) was computed for each CERQ sub-
scale .The result was shown in Table 2.
3.3. Subscales Correlations
Correlations between subscales were summarized in
Ta ble 3. The correlation matrix shows that the correla-
tions among subscales that are conceptually inverse (e.g.
Self-blame vs. other-blame, catastrophizing vs. positive
reappraisal and putting into perspective) are significantly
negative but subscales that are conceptually near (e.g.
putting into perspective, positive refocusing, positive
reappraisal and acceptance) are significantly positive.
OJPsych
3.4. Relationships between CER with Depression
and Anxiety
For investigation relationships between CER with de-
pression and anxiety was used from BDI-II and STAI.
The result of multiple regression analyses (Table 4) that
were performed with depression and anxiety scales as
dependent variables and the CERQ as independent vari-
ables shows both regression models were significant (p <
0.001) and explained 0.42 and 0.45 percentage for the
prediction of depression and anxiety, respectively. Catas-
trophizing, self-blame and rumination subscales were stro-
ngest predictors of anxiety and depression and related
with high level of anxiety and depression, but refocusing,
positive reappraisal and planning subscales related with
low level of anxiety and depression.
Table 2. Means, standard deviations and reliability of CERQ.
Test-retestα Mean (SD) Subscales
0.61 0.69 10.44 (3.52) Self-blame
0.66 0.77 7.64 (1.65) Other-blame
0.59 0.76 11.21 (4.02) Rumination
0.60 0.68 8.09 (2.45) Catastrophizing
0.65 0.80 10.61 (3.63) Planning
0.57 0.75 9.89 (3.95) Positive reappraisal
0.69 0.70 10.9 6 (2.98) Putting into perspective
0.66 0.72 8.65 (3.31) Acceptance
0.62 0.79 9.55 (3.01) Positive refocusing
Table 3. Correlations between CERQ subscales.
CERQ subscales 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Self-blame
2. Acceptance 0.21**
3. Rumination 0.17** 0.27***
4. Positive refocusing 0.08 0.24*** 0.05
5. Planning 0.40*** 0.28*** 0.33*** 0.18**
6. Positive reappraisal 0.22** 0.31*** 0.16** 0.41*** 0.44***
7. Putting into perspective 0.18** 0.27*** 0.06 0.36*** 0.38*** 0.41***
8. Catastrophizing 0.20** 0.05 0.46*** –0.07 0.03 –0.11* –0.13*
9. Other-blame –0.13* –0.02 0.25*** 0.03 0.05 –0.02 0.03 0.36***
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.
4. DISCUSSION
Table 4. Relationships between CER strategies with depression
and anxiety (multiple regression analysis)’.
The aim of this study was determination psychometric
properties of the CERQ in Iran and relationship between
CER with anxiety and depression. For study of validity
factor analysis, criterion validity and subscales correla-
tions were used. Factor analysis of CERQ yielded nine
factors that are labeled as: self-blame, other-blame, ru-
mination, catastrophizing, putting into perspective, posi-
tive refocusing, positive reappraisal, acceptance and pla-
nning. Low to moderate negative correlations among
subscales that are conceptually in versus with together
and positive correlations among subscales that are con-
ceptually near together could show fit construct validity
of this questionnaire.
STAI(Beta) BDI-II(Beta) Subscales
0.20**
0.26***
1. Self-blame
0.01
0.04
2. Acceptance
0.31***
0.23***
3. Rumination
–0.24***
–0.26***
4. Positive refocusing
–0.11*
–0.16**
5. Planning
–0.08
–0.06
6. Positive reappraisal
–0.15**
–0.12*
7. Putting into perspective
0.41***
0.32***
8.Catastrophizing
0.02
0.45***
0.06
0.42***
9. Other-blame
2
R
BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory II; STAI = State-trait Anxiety Inven-
tory; *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001. The result of internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) and
M. P. Omran / Open Journal of Psychiatry 1 (2011) 106-109 109
test-retest coefficients shows good reliability of CERQ.
Totally, these results are comparable with result of Gar-
nefski and Kraaij [4]. Accordingly, this study showed
that CERQ has fit psychometric properties and could be
used for clinical and investigative purposes in Iran.
The result of multiple regression analysis about rela-
tionship between CER strategies with depression and an-
xiety showed that CER strategies explained relatively
high amount variances of depression and anxiety. Catas-
trophizing, self-blame and rumination subscales were
related with high level of anxiety and depression, also
refocusing, positive reappraisal and planning subscales
related with low level of anxiety and depression. This
result is similar to the result of Garnefski and Kraaij [4].
The results suggest that self-blame is related to the re-
porting of symptoms of depression and anxiety. This find-
ing confirms other studies showing that an attributional
style of putting the blame of what you have experienced on
yourself is related to depression and other measures of
ill-health [17].
The results also showed a strong impact style of rumi-
nation in symptoms of depression and anxiety, fitting in
with the results of Nolen-Hoeksema et al. [18] who sho-
wed that a ruminative coping style tended to be associ-
ated with higher levels of depression. The present study
adds that rumination is also an important predictor of
symptoms of depression and anxiety.
In this article for study relationship between CER
strategies with depression and anxiety was used from
normal group. For future similar study in this field is
suggested using from clinical groups for reach to more
firm evident about relationship between CER and psych-
opathology.
REFERENCES
[1] Gross, J.J. (2001) Emotion regulation in adulthood: Tim-
ing is everything. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 10, 214-219.
doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00152
[2] Thompson, R.A. (1991) Emotional regulation and emo-
tional development. Educational Psychology Review, 3,
269-307. doi:10.1007/BF01319934
[3] Ochsner, K.N. and Gross, J.J. (2005) The cognitive con-
trol of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 242-
249. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010
[4] Garnefski, N. and Kraaij, V. (2006) Cognitive emotion
regulation questionnaire—development of a short 18-
item version (CERQ-short). Personality and Individual
Differences, 41, 1045-1053.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.04.010
[5] Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V. and Spinhoven, Ph. (2001) Ne-
gative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and de-
pression. Personality and Individual Differences, 30,
1311-1327. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00113-6
[6] Garnefski, N., van den Kommer, T., Kraaij, V., Teerds, J.,
Legerstee, J. and Onstein, E. (2002) The relationship be-
tween cognitive emotion regulation strategies and emotio-
nal problems. European Journal of Personality, 16, 403-
420. doi:10.1002/per.458
[7] Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V. and Spinhoven, Ph. (2002) Ma-
nual for the use of the cognitive emotion regulation ques-
tionnaire. The Netherlands: DATEC, Leiderdorp.
[8] Jermann, F., van der Linden, M., d’Acremont, M. and
Zermatten, A. (2006). Cognitive Emotion Regulation
(CERQ): Confirmatory factor analysis and psychometric
properties of the French translation. European Journal of
Psychological Assessment, 22, 126-131.
doi:10.1027/1015-5759.22.2.126
[9] Martin, R.C. and Dahlen, E.R. (2005) Cognitive emotion
regulation in the prediction of depression, anxiety, stress
and anger. Personality and Individual Differences, 39,
1249-1260. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.06.004
[10] D’Acremont, M. and van der Linden, M. (2006) How is
impulsivity related to depression? Evidence from a
French validation of the cognitive emotion regulation
questionnaire. Journal of Adolescence, 24, 65-75.
[11] Kraaij, V., Garnefski, N. and van Gerwen, L. (2003) Cog-
nitive coping and anxiety symptoms among people who
seek help for fear of flying. Aviation, Space and Envi-
ronmental Medicine, 74, 273-277.
[12] Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A. and Brown, G.K. (1996). Manual
for the beck depression inventory—II. Harcourt Brace &
Company San Antonio, the Psychological Corporation.
[13] Ghassemzadeh, H., Mojtabai, R. Karamghadiri, N. and
Ebrahimkhani, N. (2005) Psychometric properties of a
Persian-language version of the beck depression inven-
tory-second edition: BDI-II-PERSIAN. Depression and
Anxiety, 21, 185-192.
[14] Hollon, S.D. and Kendall, P.C. (1980) Cognitive self-
statements in depression: Development of an automatic
thoughts questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Resea-
rch, 4, 383-395. doi:10.1007/BF01178214
[15] Speilberger, C.D., Gorsuch, R.L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P.R.
and Jacobs, G.A. (1983) Manual for the state-trait anxiety
inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto.
[16] Mahram, B. (1993) Standardization speilberger state-trait
anxiety inventory in Mashad city (Iran). MA thesis of
Ferdousi University, Mashad.
[17] Anderson, C.A., Miller, R.S., Riger, A.L., Dill, J.C. and
Sedikides, C. (1994) Behavioural and characterological
styles as predictors of depression and loneliness: Review,
refinement, and test. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 66, 549-558.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.66.3.549
[18] Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Parker, L.E. and Larson, J. (1994)
Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 92-
104. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.1.92
C
opyright © 2011 SciRes. OJPsych