Vol.3, No.9, 553-556 (2011)
doi:10.4236/health.2011.39094
C
opyright © 2011 SciRes. Openly accessible at http://www.scirp.org/journal/HEALTH/
Health
Correlation between mood and heart rate variability
indices during daily life
Kohzoh Yoshino1*, Katsunori Matsuoka2
1Health Research Institute, National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology (AIST), Osaka, Japan;
*Corresponding Author: yoshino-k@aist.go.jp
2Life Science and Biotechnology, National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology (AIST), Ibaraki, Japan.
Received 1 June 2011; revised 20 July 2011; accepted 17 August 2011.
ABSTRACT
We investigated the correlation between mood
and heart rate variability (HRV) indices during
daily life. The RR-interval and body acceleration
of 40 normal male subjects were recorded using
ambulatory device for 48 to 72 hours. Every
hour that the subjects were awake they regis-
tered their current mood on a Visual Analogue
Scale questionnaire. The questionnaire scales
eight of the subjects’ current moods. Those are
happiness, tension, fatigue, worry, depression,
anger, vigor, and confusion. The following four
HRV indices were calculated. Those are heart
rate, root mean square of successive differ-
ences of RR-interval sequence, the normalized
high-frequency (0.15 - 0.4 Hz) power of RR-in-
terval variability, and mean frequency in the
high-frequency band of RR-interval variability.
The calculated HRV indices data and the mood
data were normalized individually, the data with
body acceleration exceeding 30 mG were ex-
cluded from the analysis to reduce the effect of
exercise, and the differences from the first day
(mood and HRV-index) were taken to reduce
the effect of circadian rhythm. The most three
highly correlated combinations were vigor and
HFnu (R = –0.24, p < 0.0001), vigor and
RMSSD (R = –0.24, p < 0.0001), and vigor and
HR (R = 0.22, p < 0.001). Vigor exhibited the
most significant correlations with HRV indices
of eight moods.
Keywords: Mood; Autonomic Nervous System;
Ambulatory; Heart Rate; Emotion
1. INTRODUCTION
It is important for mental health management to de-
velop a method that can assess mood during daily life in
stressful modern society. Three methods have been pro-
posed from mental state assessment. The first method is
based on subjective self-evaluation. The second is based
on biochemical analysis that quantifies the concentration
of stress-related substrates in blood plasma, urine, and
saliva. The third method is based on variability analysis
of physiological signals reflecting the state of the auto-
nomic nervous system. The third method has the fol-
lowing advantages for mental state monitoring compared
with the other two. 1) It can be measured continuously
with a short sampling interval; 2) it does not interrupt
daily life activities; 3) it can compute (assess) the values
of indices automatically; 4) it is less affected by psycho-
logical bias than the subjective self-evaluation method.
Heart rate variability (HRV) is a representative
physiological signal reflecting states of the autonomic
nervous system that can be continuously measured dur-
ing daily life with a short time interval. In order to de-
velop a method that can assess mood states from HRV, it
is necessary to understand how mood and HRV during
daily life are correlated [1]. Several studies have inves-
tigated the relationship between stress or mood and heart
rate during daily life. Dobkin and Pihl [2] demonstrated
that the heart rate of normal subjects during daily life is
significantly higher when stressed than when not
stressed. They also revealed that anxiety and hostility
contributes to the heart rate elevation. Langewitz et al.
[3] found that the perceived stress level is significantly
correlated with heart rate in normal subjects. Johnston
and Anastasiades [4] reported that heart rate during daily
life is related to arousal, stress, and time pressure in very
few normal subjects. Shapiro et al. [5] reported no cor-
relation of mood with heart rate. Sloan et al. [6] showed
that the LF/HF ratio (sympathovagal balance index) is
significantly correlated with stress (irritable, tense,
pressured, and less happy) during daily life in normal
subjects. Yoshino and Matsuoka [7] found that both
daily depression and worry shift toward sympathetic
K. Yoshino et al. / Health 3 (2011) 553-55 6
Copyright © 2011 SciRes. Openly accessible at http://www.scirp.org/journal/HEALTH/
554
dominance during subsequent sleep. Bacon et al. [8]
reported the significant association of high levels of
negative emotions (anger, stress, and sadness) with de-
creases in LF and HF powers, but higher levels of posi-
tive emotion with increases in LF power in patients with
coronary artery disease. Schwerdtfeger et al. [9] showed
that depression was related to higher heart rate through-
out the day. Moreover, there was a tendency toward
lower HRV in individuals with higher depression scores,
and this association was moderated by social context.
However, to our knowledge, no studies have investi-
gated the relationship between HRV indices and various
moods including vigor in normal subjects during daily
life. In this study, we analyzed the correlation between
four HRV indices and eight moods including vigor in
normal male subjects during daily life.
2. METHODS
Normal healthy male subjects (N = 40, age: 20 to 39
years) participated in this experiment. None of the sub-
jects had a history of cardiovascular, respiratory, or brain
disease and none were currently using any medications.
All subjects provided their informed written consent as
approved by the Ethical Committee on Human Research
at National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and
Technology. Subjects intermittently wore an ambulatory
device Active tracer (AC-301, GMS, Japan) in a pouch
at the waist for a total of 48 to 72 hours during daily life,
including sleep, except when taking a bath or shower.
This device records the RR-intervals of the electrocar-
diogram and body acceleration.
Every hour that the subjects were awake they regis-
tered their current mood on a 100-mm-long Visual Ana-
logue Scale (VAS) questionnaire on which the end-
points were labeled “lowest” and “highest”. The ques-
tionnaire scales eight of the subjects’ current moods:
happiness, tension, fatigue, worry, depression, anger,
vigor, and confusion. We instructed subjects not to an-
swer the questionnaire more than 15 minutes after the
predetermined time, and we strictly prohibited subjects
from recalling a past mood when answering the ques-
tionnaire.
The following four HRV indices 1) to 4) were calcu-
lated from RR-interval sequence data collected during
10 min before answering the VAS questionnaires if at
least 96% of 10-min RR-interval records were within
valid range (0.3 - 2.0 sec). The RR-intervals out of this
range were omitted from the analysis. If the mean body
acceleration in the 10-min exceeded 30 mG, the corre-
sponding RR-interval data were excluded from the
analysis in order to exclude the effect of body movement.
It has been reported that 157.5 mG is the optimum value
of threshold to discriminate activity state and rest by
body acceleration [10]. We used the value 30 mG which
is five times smaller than it in this study. The inter-indi-
vidual mean excluded ratio was 47.2% ± 12.5%, and
there was no significant difference between before and
after noon.
1) Heart rate (HR) and 2) root mean square of succes-
sive differences (RMSSD) of RR-interval sequence were
calculated for each 10-min data set. HR takes low values
and RMSSD takes high values when the balance of
autonomic nervous system activity (sympathovagal bal-
ance) shifts toward parasympathetic dominance.
The following frequency analysis was performed in
addition to the time domain analysis. The power spec-
trum was calculated by applying a fast Fourier transform
(FFT) to the RR-interval signal for each 10-min data set.
The area (i.e., power) of high-frequency components
(HF: 0.15 to 0.4 Hz) of the power spectrum was divided
by the total power from which the very-low-frequency
component (VLF: <0.04 Hz) had been subtracted in or-
der to calculate (3) the normalized HF power (HFnu),
which takes higher values when the sympathovagal bal-
ance shifts toward parasympathetic dominance. 4) Mean
frequency in HF band of the power spectrum (MFHF)
were calculated. MFHF is related to respiratory fre-
quency.
To reduce inter-individual variability, we normalized
the measured values of mood level and HRV indices by
dividing by their amplitudes (maximum-minimum in
measurement) after subtracting their minimum values for
each subject. Moreover, to reduce the effect of circadian
rhythm, the differences of the normalized values of
mood level and HRVI indices on the second or third day
from those at the same time of the first (reference) day
(denoted as mood and HRVI) were calculated for
each subject.
Before applying the correlation analysis, we con-
firmed that the inter-individual differences of mood
and HRVI variations were not relatively high. This was
done by comparing the inter-individual means and stan-
dard deviations of mood and HRVI variations. Fol-
lowing the confirmation, Pearson’s correlation coeffi-
cients between eight moods and four HRVIs were
calculated.
3. RESULTS
The comparison results are summarized in Table 1.
The most three highly correlated combinations were
vigor and HFnu (R = –0.24, p < 0.0001) (Figure 1
right), vigor and RMSSD (R = –0.24, p < 0.0001),
and vigor and HR (R = 0.22, p < 0.001) (Figure 1
left). There were three combinations whose p-values
were less than 0.001 (Table 1). All of them were com-
binations with vigor. This indicates that vigor has the
K. Yoshino et al. / Health 3 (2011) 553-55 6
Copyright © 2011 SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/HEALTH/
555555
Table 1. Pearson’s correlation coefficients between four HRVIs and eight moods.
Δhappiness Δtension Δfatigue Δworry Δdepression Δanger Δvigor Δconfusion
ΔHR 0.11 0.05 –0.02 0.05 0.05 0.01 0.22** 0.03
ΔRMSSD –0.19* –0.07 0.08 –0.03 0.00 –0.01 –0.24*** –0.05
ΔHFnu –0.03 –0.13 –0.07 –0.05 –0.04 –0.10 –0.24*** –0.10
ΔMFHF –0.12 –0.08 –0.11 –0.06 –0.01 0.01 –0.12 –0.00
*: p < 0.01, **: p < 0.001, ***: p < 0.0001.
Figure 1. Scatter plots of Δvigor versus ΔHR (left) and Δvigor versus ΔHFnu (right).
most significant correlations with HRV indices of eight
moods.
Openly accessible at
4. DISCUSSION
The significant negative correlations between vigor
and HFnu (R = –0.24, p < 0.0001) and between vigor
and RMSSD (R = –0.24, p < 0.0001) and the signifi-
cant positive correlation between vigor and HR (R =
0.22, p < 0.001) suggest that vigor during daily life shifts
the autonomic nervous system balance toward sympa-
thetic dominance.
Russell proposed a circumplex model that defines
emotion space by pleasure-displeasure (valence) axis
and arousal-sleep (arousal) axis [11]. Thayer decom-
posed the arousal axis into two dimensions based on the
results from factor analysis [12,13]. One is tense arousal
and the other is energetic arousal. The tense arousal
represents a continuum from calmness to anxiety,
whereas the energetic arousal reflects a continuum from
tiredness to energy. Dickman broke down the energetic
arousal still further into two dimensions of wakefulness
and vigor [14]. The result from the presented study im-
plies that autonomic nervous system (HRV) indices co-
variate with vigor the fourth dimension axis in emotion
space during daily life.
In our previous study, we demonstrated that both daily
depression and worry shift toward sympathetic domi-
nance during subsequent sleep, whereas vigor had little
effect on the autonomic nervous system balance during
subsequent sleep [7]. The results in this study and in our
previous study imply that vigor during daily life concur-
rently has a relatively strong effect on shifting the auto-
nomic nervous system balance toward sympathetic
dominance, but the effect is not sufficiently long-lasting
to make an effect during subsequent sleep.
There are several limitations pertain to this study.
First, the effects of circadian rhythm on mood and HRVI
have not been completely removed, although we took
the differences of the values of indices on the second or
third day from those at the same time of the first (refer-
ence) day. This is because of the day-to-day variation in
the circadian rhythm of mood and autonomic nervous
activity.
Second, the result of this study can be applied to only
half of the daily awaking time. This is because about
half (47.2%) of the case was excluded from the analysis,
since the mean body acceleration was over 30 mG.
In summary, we demonstrated that vigor during daily
K. Yoshino et al. / Health 3 (2011) 553-55 6
Copyright © 2011 SciRes. Openly accessible at http://www.scirp.org/journal/HEALTH/
556
life has the most significant correlations with HRV indi-
ces (autonomic nervous system balance) of eight moods.
5. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This work was supported by Grant-in-aid for Scientific Research
(KAKENHI) for Young Scientists (B) (No.19700557) from the Minis-
try of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology, Japan.
REFERENCES
[1] Kreibig, S.D. (2010) Autonomic nervous system activity
in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology, 84, 394-
421. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010
[2] Dobkin, P.L. and Pihl, R.O. (1992) Measurement of psy-
chological and heart rate reactivity to stress in the real
world. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 58, 204-214.
doi:10.1159/000288629
[3] Langewitz, W., Ruddel, H. and Von Eiff, A.W. (1987)
Influence of perceived level of stress upon ambulatory
blood pressure, heart rate, and respiratory frequency.
Journal of Clinical Hypertension, 3, 743-748.
[4] Johnston, D.W. and Anastasiades, P. (1990) The rela-
tionship between heart rate and mood in real life. Journal
of Psychosomatic Research, 34, 21-27.
doi:10.1016/0022-3999(90)90004-N
[5] Shapiro, D., Jamner, L.D. and Goldstein, I.B. (1997)
Daily mood states and ambulatory blood pressure. Psy-
chophysiology, 34, 399-405.
doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02383.x
[6] Sloan, R.P., Shapiro, P.A., Bagiella, E., Boni, S.M., Paik,
M., Bigger, J.T. Jr., Steinman, R.C. and Gorman, J.M.
(1994) Effect of mental stress throughout the day on car-
diac autonomic control. Biological Psychology, 37, 89-99.
doi:10.1016/0301-0511(94)90024-8
[7] Yoshino, K. and Matsuoka, K. (2009) Effect of mood
during daily life on autonomic nervous activity balance
during subsequent sleep. Autonomic Neuroscience, 150,
147-149. doi:10.1016/j.autneu.2009.03.013
[8] Bacon, S.L., Watkins, L.L., Babyak, M., Sherwood, A.,
Hayano, J., Hinderliter, A.L., Waugh, R. and Blumenthal,
J.A. (2004) Effects of daily stress on autonomic cardiac
control in patients with coronary artery disease. Ameri-
can Journal of Cardiology, 93, 1292-1294.
doi:10.1016/j.amjcard.2004.02.018
[9] Schwerdtfeger, A. and Friedrich-Mai, P. (2009) Social
interaction moderates the relationship between depres-
sive mood and heart rate variability: Evidence from an
ambulatory monitoring study. Health Psychology, 28,
501-509. doi:10.1037/a0014664
[10] Mathie, M.J., Coster, A.C.F., Lovell, N.H. and Celler,
B.G. (2003) Detection of daily physical activities using a
triaxial accelerometer. Medical & Biological Engineering
& Computing, 41 , 296-301. doi:10.1007/BF02348434
[11] Russell, J.A. (1979) Affective space is bipolar. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 345-356.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.37.3.345
[12] Thayer, R.E. (1967) Measurement of activation through
self-report. Psychological Reports, 20, 663-678.
[13] Thayer, R.E. (1978) Factor analytic and reliability studies
on the Activation-Deactivation Adjective Checklist.
Psychological Reports, 42, 747-756.
[14] Dickman, S.J. (2002) Dimensions of arousal: Wakeful-
ness and vigor. Human Factors, 44, 429-442.
doi:10.1518/0018720024497673