Psychology
2011. Vol.2, No.3, 216-219
Copyright © 2011 SciRes. DOI:10.4236/psych.2011.23033
Memory Strength and Criterion Shift in the False Memory
Paradigm: A Learning Case
Shahid Naved, Ameer Haider Ali, Khubaib Ahmed Qureshi
Hamdard University, Karachi, Pakistan.
Email: khubaib_ahmed@yahoo.com
Received February 28th, 2011; revised April 15th, 2011; accepted May 18th, 2011.
The attempt has been made to investigate the criterion shift hypothesis once again by re-evaluating the confi-
dence measurement, which will possibly clarify the role that criterion shifts play in the false memory phenome-
non (recollection of an event, or the details of an event, that did not occur). Literature review shows that this
hypothesis still needs research upon the same topic. The study was experimental in which students of Hamdard
University were selected as subjects40 students from BBA and MBA programs. Both male/female and
left/right handed subjects participated. All the subjects were not native English speakers. The experiment was
conducted using a computer program to collect the data. The experiment had two parts, firstly a study/recall
phase and secondly a test/recognition phase. The scale we introduced to allow participants to assess their own
certainty about the classification of recognition items is more detailed than that used in the Roediger and
McDermott study. Our hypothesis was that a shift in decision criterion would become evident by means of a
lower certainty measure for lure words as compared to target words from the lists. This difference was found in
our data. The mean certainty measure we found for the critical lures is significantly lower than the mean cer-
tainty for the targets.
Keywords: Memory, Recognition, Lures, Associate Memory
Introduction
Roediger and McDermott (1995), by applying methods first
introduced by Deese (1959), have investigated the false mem-
ory phenomenon. In their study, subjects learned lists of words
strongly related to one non-presented word. In recognition and
recall tests, the non-presented strong association was recog-
nized and recalled at a rate comparable to actually presented
words. Specifically, in their first experiment, the non-presented
associates were recalled 40% of the time. The authors have
concluded that this procedure reliably creates false memories.
They put a heavy emphasis on the illusionary character of the
phenomenon, comparable to perceptual illusions. Additionally,
the effect could also be observed in cases where the subjects
were specifically informed that one would try to induce false
memories in them. Roediger and McDermott provided some
speculative explanatory approaches, one being that the false
memory experience is the result of a reconstructive process that,
in the case of auditory encoding, makes it possible for the sub-
ject to generate a representation of how the word would have
sounded if presented in the speaker’s voice. This clear repre-
sentation would make it plausible that the subjects wrongly
claim to remember the word’s presentation.
Miller and Wolford (1999) have reliably reproduced the
findings of Roediger and McDermott. In their opinion, however,
the high rates of false recall and recognition are due to a change
in the decision criterion. When asked to rate a presented item as
new/old, the strength of that particular item is generally be-
lieved to be made up of two factors. Firstly, the item gains evi-
dence of having been seen before through presentation. Addi-
tionally, however, the item also gains strength due to associa-
tive activation, meaning that an item semantically related to the
list items will be considered more likely to be old than unre-
lated items. If the strength of activation for a particular item
exceeds a certain threshold, it is considered to be old; otherwise
it will be classified as new. Yet, according to Miller and
Wolford (1999), the false memory phenomenon thus observed
does not really represent false memory. In their opinion, a
change in decision criteria, thus in the threshold for old/new
classification, is responsible for the phenomenon. More spe-
cifically, for the critical lures Miller and Wolford claim that the
decision criterion is lower than for unrelated lures, because
subjects judge the critical lures to be more likely to be old due
to the fact that they are strongly semantically related to the rest
of the list.
The thesis of Miller and Wolford has been criticized, for in-
stance by Wixted and Stretch (2000). Even though the claim
that the false memory phenomenon is only due to criterion shift
is quite controversial, even Wixted and Stretch admit that it is
uncertain if and to what extent criterion shifts contribute to
false memory. Further research is needed to answer this open
question.
The objective of this experiment is to test the criterion shift
hypothesis once again. We think that the introduction of a con-
fidence measurement could possibly clarify the role that crite-
rion shifts play in the false memory phenomenon. Thus, when
asked to judge an item, subjects will not only be asked to clas-
sify that item as being either new or old, but they will also be
asked to rate their own confidence about their decision on a
scale. If there is a change in the decision criterion during rec-
ognition of items and lures from a given list, then the rate of
false alarm for critical lures should be the same as in the
Roediger/McDermott experiment. However, the confidence
ratings for those items should generally be lower than the con-
S. NAVED ET AL. 217
fidence/memory strength ratings for presented items. This is,
after all, what is meant by a change in decision criterion: items
are judged to be old even though they have a lower activation
because the threshold for a positive decision is lower. Similar
measurements have been taken in the experiments by Roedi-
ger/McDermott and Miller/Wolford, but with a scaling that is
too rough to properly examine the distribution of memory
strength values.
Thus this experimental hypothesis is that criterion shifts
should become evident in our setting by means of lower mem-
ory strength/confidence measures for critical lures.
Methodology
Subjects
Due to the nature of the experiment, many more students
wanted to participate, however 40 subjects were randomly se-
lected from BBA and MBA programs. The ages ranged be-
tween 18 - 25 years, included both male/female and left/right
handed subjects participated. All subjects were not native Eng-
lish speakers.
Materials
Six lists of items by consulting the data from “Norms of
word association” by Russell (1954) were created. Every list
contained the 12 associates with the highest frequency from one
of Russell’s lists. We created seven recognition items per list,
one of which was the main associate of the original list. Among
these recognition items there were 2 unrelated items. These
were picked arbitrarily from the unused lists of Russell (1954).
Also, two weak associates were part of the recognition items
for each particular list. Those weak associates were taken from
the positions 13 and lower of the association norms. Finally,
two of the recognition items were studied words from the list.
One of these was the first item in the association norms list, the
other occurred somewhere in the first six positions. The recog-
nition items were presented in blocks of seven items, each
block corresponding to one studied list. One block always
started with a studied word and ended with the critical lure. The
order of the blocks corresponded to the order in which the lists
had been studied.
Procedure
The experiment was conducted using a computer program to
collect the data. Generally, the experiment had two parts, firstly
a study/recall phase and secondly a test/recognition phase.
During study phase, the lists were presented to the Subjects on
a computer screen one word at a time, each word presentation
lasting about 2 seconds. After the presentation of each list there
was a recall test, in which the Subjects were instructed to write
down as many words as they could remember from the previous
presentation. This process was repeated six times, once per list.
After the completion of the study/recall phase, there was a short
break, after which the recognition test took place. The instruc-
tions for this recognition test were displayed on each Subject's
computer screen. Then the recognition items were presented
one at a time in the order described in the materials section. For
each item, Subjects first had to decide whether it was a studied
item or an unstudied one. In both cases, they afterwards had to
rate their confidence on a scale of 1 (absolutely sure) ... 7 (not
sure at all).
At the end of the experiment, Subjects were given handouts
explaining the purpose of the experiment in which they had just
participated.
Results
We collected data for the old/new classification and for the
certainty measure. As expected, a high probability for “old”
classification was found for both lures and targets.
The certainty and old/new data was then compiled onto one
scale by multiplying the certainty value by –1 for words classi-
fied as “new” and by 1 for words classified as “old”. Thus, the
resulting scale ranged from –7 (absolutely sure the item is new)
to +7 (absolutely sure the item is old).
From Table 2 it can be seen that a difference in the mean
certainty for lure words and for targets was found.
The frequency distributions of the certainty values were also
computed, separately for each recognition item type:
Subsequently the standard error for both the mean certainty
values and the mean probability for an “old” response were
computed (Figure 5, Figure 6). Here the standard error of the
mean is indicated by the vertical lines through the data points.
A significant difference in mean certainty and mean probability
of old classification was found between targets and lure words.
Discussion
First of all, we reproduced the findings of Roediger/
Table 1.
Mean probability of old classification.
Item Type Mean P (Old)
Lures 0.7
Target 0.88
Weakly related 0.09
Unrelated 0.04
Table 2.
Mean certainty measure after rescal i ng (Scale from 7 to 7).
Item Type Mean Certainty
Lures 2.88
Target 5.05
Weakly related 4.34
Unrelated 5.23
S. NAVED ET AL.
218
Figure 1.
Histogram lure items certainty.
Figure 2.
Histogram target item certainty.
Figure 3.
Histogram weekly related items certainty.
McDermott (1995). The false alarm rates found for critical non-
studied lure words were almost as high as in the Roedi-
ger/McDermott study. The scale we introduced to allow par-
ticipants to assess their own certainty about the classification of
recognition items is more detailed than that used in the Roedi-
ger/McDermott study (1995).
Figure 4.
Histogram unrelated item certainty text with figure.
Figure 5.
Standard error of mean P (old) per item type.
Figure 6.
Standard error of me a n c e r ta i n t y p e r i t e m t y p e.
Our hypothesis was that a shift in decision criterion would
become evident by means of a lower certainty measure for lure
words as compared to target words from the lists. This differ-
ence was found in our data. The mean certainty measure we
found for the critical lures is significantly lower than the mean
certainty for the targets. The reason for this is most probably
S. NAVED ET AL. 219
the range of our certainty scale, since similar measurements
with a rougher scaling were taken for instance by Roediger/
McDermott (1995) and no such effect was found.
The question remains what this means for the ongoing dis-
cussion about the role that criterion shift plays in the false
memory phenomenon.
Conclusion
The fact that lower certainty measures could actually be col-
lected for critical lures indicates that criterion shift cannot be
ignored when discussing the false memory phenomenon. This
implies that it is very unlikely that associative models alone can
account for the false memory phenomenon.
At the same time, our results provide no further information
concerning the role of associative processes in the phenomenon.
Further research examining the exact mechanisms at work here
is needed to clarify the role that these processes play in the
phenomenon. Thus, it can by no means be ruled out that in fact
an interaction of associative mechanisms and mechanisms of
shifts in decision criterion result in the false memory effect.
Possibly the effect we found is weaker than it actually could
have been. This is because a number of participants reported
that they found the scaling range from 1 (not sure) to 7 (abso-
lutely sure) confusing. Thus it can be assumed that a number of
them made minor rating mistakes while assessing their certainty.
References
Deese, J. (1995). Influence of inter-item associative strength upon
immediate free recall. Psychological Reports, 5, 305-312.
doi:10.2466/PR0.5.3.305-312
Miller, M. B., & Wolford, G. L. (1999). Theoretical commentary: The
role of criterion shift in false memory. Psychological Review, 106,
398-405. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.106.2.398
McDermott, K. B., & Roediger H. L. (1998). Attempting to avoid
illusory memories: Robust false recognition of associates persists
under conditions of explicit warnings and immediate testing. Journal
of Memory and Language, 39, 508-520. doi:10.1006/jmla.1998.2582
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott K. B. (1995). Creating false memories:
Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21 , 803-814.
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott K. B. (1999). False alarms and false
memories. Psychological Review, 106, 406-410.
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.106.2.406
Russell (1954). Norms of word association. L. Postman (Ed.), 1970,
New York: Academic Press.
Smith, R. E., & Hunt R. R. (1999). Presentation modality affects false
memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5, 710-715.
doi:10.3758/BF03208850
Rhodes, M. G., & Anastasi J. S. (2000). The effects of levels of proc-
essing manipulation on false recall. Psychonomic Bulletin and Re-
view, 7, 158-162. doi:10.3758/BF03210735
Wixted, J. T., & Stretch V. (2000). The case against a criterion shift
account of false memory. Psychological Review, 107, 368-376.
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.2.368