Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science
Vol.4 No.1(2014), Article ID:42435,10 pages DOI:10.4236/jbbs.2014.41005

Reviewing Self-Injuries Behavior in Macaques: The Role of Early Experience and Genetics in an Integrated Non-Human Model

Juan D. Molina1,2*, Mario de la Calle Real3, Alfonso Ramos Ruiz2, Francisco López-Muñoz2, Maria José Muñoz Algar1, Cristina Andrade-Rosa4, F. Toledo-Romero5

1Acute Inpatients Unit, Dr. R. Lafora Psychiatric Hospital, Madrid, Spain

2Health Sciences Faculty, Camilo José Cela University, Madrid, Spain

3Instituto Psiquiátrico Montreal, Hospital de Día, Madrid, Spain

4Centro de Salud Los Castillos, Madrid, Spain

5“Virgen de la Arrixaca” Clinical University Hospital, Murcia, Spain

Email: *candrader@medynet.com

Copyright © 2014 Juan D. Molina et al. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. In accordance of the Creative Commons Attribution License all Copyrights © 2014 are reserved for SCIRP and the owner of the intellectual property Juan D. Molina et al. All Copyright © 2014 are guarded by law and by SCIRP as a guardian.

Received November 17, 2013; revised December 26, 2013; accepted January 9, 2014

Keywords:5-HTT; Primates; Allelic Variation; Diathesis-Stress Model; Self-Injurious Behavior; SIB

ABSTRACT

Genetic differences in non-human primates and the context where they were reared are important in the determination of their behaviors. Some studies suggest that Self-Injurious Behavior results from stress or anxiety, but some macaques appear to be more vulnerable to acquiring Self-Injurious Behavior. Vulnerability and risk for socially reared macaques of developing Self-Injurious Behavior increased when they experienced early adverse events and suffered from consequent stress. Stressful events can cause alterations in the neuroendocrine and neuropeptide systems associated with the regulation of stress and anxiety. Dysregulation in these systems contributes to the occurrence of anxious episodes that lead to self-harm. This could be used by those animals to counteract the effects of anxiety, causing euphoria by releasing endogenous opioids. In developing an integrative approach of Self-Injurious Behavior, we propose the diathesis-stress model to illustrate how the environment and genetics are influenced as well, taking rhesus monkeys as an example. Rhesus macaques and humans have essentially the same gene 5-HTT as well as functional polymorphism. The allelic variation in the region promoting the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTT) was associated with significant differences in the number of depressive symptoms and contributions to social reward and punishment. Growing up without the mother and the rearing environment are important risk factors for developing abnormal behaviors. The main idea is that monkeys who carry short 5-HTT allele and are reared with their peers (without their mother) suffer from neurobiological developmental delay, deteriorated serotonergic functioning and act more aggressively. Monkeys who carry short 5-HIAA allele and are reared with their mothers, though, show normal behavioral patterns. We could conclude that carrying the short allele of gene 5-HTT leads to a psychopathological tendency in macaques with a poor parenting history, but it may be adaptive for those who establish an attached relationship with their mothers. Stressful early experiences result in impaired 5-HTT expression as well. There are both specific genetic and environmental factors which pose a risk in the development of adverse responses to stress and environment. Rhesus macaques, as well as humans, show big individual differences in their responses to environmental stress. Some are very fearful and anxious, others are aggressive and impulsive. It is possible to identify genetic and environmental patterns that contribute to these differences, but recent evidence suggests that the interaction between genes and environment is of equal importance.

1. Introduction

“Self-injurious behavior” (SIB) is understood as any self-inflicted act causing tissue damages [1]. They can occur as cuts, burns, bites, etc. [2], although it doesn’t require an injury to categorize an action as self-injurious when there is a potential danger [3].

SIB is often used to reduce stress or anxiety [4-9], whereas stress management was identified as one of the reasons for the appearance of this behavior in 77% of a clinical sample that had suffered a previous trauma [10]. Furthermore, Symons et al. [11] found a significant positive correlation between salivary cortisol levels and the severity of SIB in adults with developmental disabilities, supporting the possible relationship between stress and self-harm.

This behavior is not exclusive to humans. A small percentage of monkeys in captivity also showed them in a study [12,13]. In fact, an animal model that has been studied extensively is the rhesus macaques as it is not based on any principle of social isolation or biological or chemical manipulation. Thus it is more likely that they share with humans some of the etiological and physiopathological factors that contribute to the development of self-harm [14]. In addition, macaques and humans share between 90 and 94 percent of their genetics [15] and have a similar endocrine and neurophysiological system as well as social complexes, hierarchies and environmental experiences [16,17]. Another reason why the animal model is useful when considering SIB is that their environment can be controlled; thus the non-human primates provide a valid model of understanding the origins of the interaction between early adverse experiences and functional genetic variants in the development of stressrelated disorders [18,19].

To understand the singularity of the example of rhesus macaques, it is noteworthy that studies with other family members of macaques show no functional polymorphism of the 5-HTT gene [20] or MAO-A [21]. Furthermore, another characteristic shared only by humans and rhesus macaques is that they can live in a wide range of habitats and social environments, presenting great adaptabilities [22].

2. Biological Correlates

It is important to understand what biological correlations there are behind SIB and its utility to the subject. Thus, on a physiological and neurochemical level, some studies indicate that macaques exhibiting SIB have long-term alterations in the central and peripheral opiods and in their stress-response-systems, which causes higher levels of anxiety [14]. It seems that there are various biological systems implicated in the expression and upholding of this kind of behavior in humans, including the endocrine and the central monoamine systems [23,24]. Specifically identified to play an important role in the expression of self-harm have been dysregulations in central serotonergic neurotransmission (5-HT) [25,26] and/or dopaminergic [27-30], and also central opioid and peripheric systems [26,31,32].

Some studies suggest that SIB in socially reared macaques is associated with complex changes in the function of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal (HPA). It is also noteworthy that plasma cortisol levels correlated negatively with the frequency of self-biting, which is suggesting a connection between the main expression of the disorder (self-biting) and observed HPA dysregulation [33].

There are two main hypotheses about central and peripheral opioids [23,24,26,34,35]. The hypothesis of self-administration of opiates—or hypothesis of addiction—indicates that individuals with SIB believe that they are addicted to the release of endogenous opioids and for that reason they harm themselves [34,36]. The second hypothises—called the pain hypothesis—argues that a high endogenous opioid activity increases the pain threshold (hypoalgesia) [37]. Thus they feel no pain during SIB and possibly do it as a form of self-stimulation [34]. It has been shown that rhesus macaques, which exhibit self-harm, direct their biting to body parts related to analgesia, produced by acupuncture or acupressure [38].

Furthermore, most of the related studies with animals and humans suggest that an increased activity of the noradrenergic system gives occasion to aggressive behavior. Additionally, a downregulation of the density of postsynaptic noradrenergic receptors in the prefrontal cortex has been demonstrated in postmortem tissue of suicides, possibly happening after an increased release of this neurotransmitter [39].

Gene-Environement Interaction

Researchers such as Caspi, Moffit and colleagues demonstrated that the allelic variation in the region promoting the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTT) was associated with significant differences in the number of depressive symptoms, but only if there was neglect or abuse during childhood or if the subject was experiencing high levels of current stress [40]. Another survey shows that genetic variation in serotonin function contributes to social reward and punishment in rhesus macaques, and thus shapes social behavior [41]. Rhesus macaques and humans have essentially the same gene 5-HTT as well as functional polymorphism [42].

Several studies have shown that individual differences in concentrations of 5-HIAA are highly hereditary between macaques of similar age and upbringing [43]. Moreover, both types of 5-HIAA concentrations in the cerebrospinal fluid and its corresponding behavioral correlation are due to significant modifications through early social experiences, particularly those related to attachment [44].

Recent researches have demonstrated significant interactions between specific genetic factors and life experience in rhesus macaques. For example the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTT) has a length variation resulting in an allelic variation in hisexpression. A heterozygous short allele (long-short) provides a low efficiency of the transcription to the 5-HTT promoter compared to the homozygous long allele (long-long) being more likely that a low expression of the gene can result in a decrease in the serotonergic function [45]. Also there are researches which suggest that polymorphisms in the gene encoding the 5-HT transporter modulate the adverse consequences of social subordination on the timing of puberty in female rhesus monkeys [46]. Polymorphisms in this gene may even contribute to differences in reproductive compromise in response to chronic stress [47].

Early environments involving factors such as childhood physical/sexual abuse may prove useful for explanation of the personality disorders concept based on gene-environment interaction [48]. Many studies argue that the expression of 5-HTT is relevant to the relationship between early life stress and impulsive behavior, holding even this relationship is observable at an early stage in the development of rhesus macaques [49]. A body of literature suggests that stressful early experiences result in impaired 5-HTT expression [50-54].

Macaques being carriers of the short allele of the 5-HTT gene show an early retardation in their neurobiological development, deteriorated serotonin function and excessive aggressiveness, but only if they were bred with their peers without their biological mother. Those who are raised with their biological mother and carry this allele show normal development patterns [55-58]. In addition, the monkeys reared by their mothers who carry the short allele consume less alcohol than those reared by their mothers but carrying the long allele, which tells us that carrying the less efficient allele can be a risk factor for monkeys raised without their mothers, but a protective factor for monkeys raised by their biological mothers [59].

A similar pattern occurs in relation to aggressiveness: macaques reared with their peers with long-short allele showed high levels of aggression while those reared by their mothers carrying the same allele were compared with long-short allele (both those raised by their mothers and those raised by their peers) suggesting, again, a compensatory effect of maternal parenting [60].

Champoux et al. [61] found out that infant macaques that carried the long-short allele and were reared in laboratories without their mothers showed significant deficits in attention, activity and motor maturity in relation to those raised as well without their mother but carrying the long-long allele. On the other hand, infant monkeys with long-short allele and long-long being reared by their biological mother showed normal values [61].

We could conclude that carrying the short allele of gene 5-HTT leads to a psychopathology in macaques with a poor parenting history, but it may be adaptive for those who establish an attached relationship with their mothers [44].

Another gene whose allelic variation has been studied in relation to the interaction between genes and the environment and the presence or absence of a history of rejection and/or abuse in childhood is the MAO-A. An interpretation of Caspi and colleagues about the polymorphism of the MAO-A is that the most efficient allele protects individuals, who carry it, from possible effects of aggressive behavior resulting from early adverse experiences like rejection or abuse in childhood [40]. In other words: a “good” gene offers protection against a “bad” environment [20].

Gathering all the data from these studies, it seems clear that there are specific genetic and environmental factors which pose a risk in the development of adverse responses to stress and environment [44]. Some authors suggest that genetic or environmental factors may mitigate the risk for behavioral dysregulation illustrated in the patterns of behavioral activity and emotional reactivity displayed by infants [62].

3. Diathesis-Stress Model

In developing an integrative approach of SIB we propose the diathesis-stress model as a psychological theory that can explain this behavior as a result of genetic vulnerability together with stress from life experiences from an evolutionary point of view.

3.1. Vulnerability: The Role of Early Experiences in the Diathesis-Stress Model

Some macaques appear to be more vulnerable to acquiring SIB. Data obtained through the records of a rhesus macaque colony suggests that this higher vulnerability and risk for socially reared macaques of developing SIB increases when they experienced early adverse events and suffered from consequent stress [14]. Monkeys with SIB also have a dysregulation of the HPA axis, indicated by a blunted cortisol response to mild stressors [63].

Likewise, Tiefenbacher et al. [33] consider that some animals could be predetermined for the development of SIB and, at the same time, exhibit alterations in the function of the HPA system. On the other hand, it seems that stressful life experiences such as early separation from the parents or numerous veterinary consultations are risk factors for the development of this behavior [63-65].

Several factors influence the development of abnormal behavior in animals in captivity, but an interruption in their early upbringing seems to be the most important contribution to the development of behavioral problems [3,63,66-69]. Individuals raised by a nanny seem more prone to develop abnormal behavior than those reared by their mothers [3,63].

Growing up without the mother and the rearing environment are important risk factors for developing abnormal behaviors [3,70,71]. Some studies suggest that some of these behaviors may be established before the socialization begins. Thus, the lack of early tactile stimulation and responses to distress in childhood may be factors in the development of abnormal behaviors [71].

Other studies indicate that there are further risk factors for the development of self-harm [3,63,66,72]. In the case of self-inflicted wounds, several of these factors are related to individual cages, the age at which they have been caged for the first time [3,63,72]and the total caging duration [3,72].

Lutz et al. [3], following researches conducted decades ago by Harlow and Harlow [73], argue that the best predictor of SIB is a poor early rearing experience. Monkeys brought up without the company of members of their species during the first six months of their life developed generalized patterns of anomalies known as “isolation syndrome”, which comprise a more emotional, inappropriate social behavior, inadaptability to new situations and an extensive repertoire of behavior disorders [74].

Another factor to consider in the expression of selfharm is the exposure to stressful events, which can oscillate from an increase of the sound level, altering the sense of balance in chimpanzees [75], up to changes in breeding schemes, which can cause an increase in stereotyped movements in macaques and squirrel monkeys [76,77].

3.2. Stress and/or Anxiety

As in humans [78-80], anxiety is an important factor in the expression of SIB in monkeys reared in society according to results of studies in colonies. Treatment with Diazepam produced a significant decrease in self-inflicted bites and the occurrence of injuries in half of the monkeys. However, this treatment increased the occurrence of injuries in the remaining animals from New England Primate Research Center (NEPRC) colonies that were also treated with Diazepam [81]. The evaluation of the records of the colony indicated that animals with a positive response to the treatment had spent more time individually housed and had experienced a greater number of minor veterinary interventions than those who had a negative response. These results imply the existence of two types of monkey populations with SIB: those whose behavior arises from life experiences and who are sensitive to treatment with anxiolytic drugs, and those whose behavior is related to other factors (i.e. genetic) and who do not respond to anxiolytic treatment [14].

A probable stressor for these animals, in addition to captivity, is the relocation of their homes, as several studies have shown that this causes a cortisol response [82]. A study by Davenport et al. [2] with rhesus macaques shows that there are behavioral and physiological changes in male monkeys who suffered from stress when relocated. In fact, the stressor caused a pathological response in animals with particularly strong SIB, significantly increasing the frequency of disruptive self-bites and sleep disorders. The results show that this behavior may be exacerbated by stress in this animal model and those stressful events lead to the development and maintenance of this pathological behavior [12,14,83].

3.3. Individual Differences

Not everybody responds the same way to similar stimuli. It has been found about 20% of the monkeys react to not very stressful social situations with an unusual pattern of fear and anxiety, accompanied by a prolonged activation of the HPA axis. In turn, about 10% of the population shows impulsive and/or aggressive behavior under similar circumstances, demonstrating a dysfunction in serotonergic activity [20].

But how do these differences develop? Rhesus macaques grown in the wild spend the first month almost exclusively with their mother. In their second month, the hatchlings begin to explore their physical and social environment, using their mother as a “safety base”. In the subsequent weeks and months, they are spending more time away from their mothers, beginning to build relationships with other group members (mostly of their own age). Throughout the rest of their childhood, they are spending several hours a day in this social game with peers, what is important in the development of a normal functioning adulthood. Aggressive hatchlings often show violent behavior during this social game, starting combat games that end in physical violence against their fellow players. Both the anxious and the aggressive rhesus macaques tend to show significant deviations from this pattern of social development starting at a very early point in their lives. Anxious hatchlings begin to separate from their mothers much later and show a lower ratio of exploratory behavior than the rest of their peers. They spend less time playing with other macaques of the same age, and when they are physically separated from their mother, they demonstrate a high degree of anxiety, accompanied by high and prolonged levels of cortisol [20,84]. Several studies show a significant genetic component to that [85,86].

Parenting Schemes

Macaques reared without their mother developed a strong attachment among them in only a few days. This attachment is not functional because a peer is not able to fulfill maternal functions (protection, calm, etc.) [87]. Additionally, those reared without their biological mother showed less significantly concentration of 5-HIAA in the cerebrospinal fluid from childhood to early adulthood [88,89].

We can conclude that macaques reared without their biological mother show the same behavior and biological patterns as a response to the environmental and social stress as anxious and aggressive monkeys. Early social experiences have consequences for the rhesus macaques both in their biological development and behavior, far beyond genetics [20].

4. Conclusions

Because a brain in development shows more plasticity and responses to environmental changes than an adult brain [90], early detection may be the best strategy as a remedy. In any case, a combination of social enrichment, use of positive reinforcement, increasing control of an animal’s environment and improving parenting techniques for the ones without their biological mother may be the best solutions for these anomalous behaviors [71].

Since there is no hypothesis that can fully explain SIB, Tiefenbacher et al. [14] propose an integrated hypothesis of the origin and maintenance. It suggests that early adverse experiences, followed by stressful events, can cause alterations in the neuroendocrine and neuropeptide systems associated with the regulation of stress and anxiety. Dysregulation in these systems contributes to the occurrence of anxious episodes that leads to self-harm. This could be used by those animals to counteract the effects of anxiety, causing euphoria by releasing endogenous opioids.

We would like to add that rhesus macaques, as well as humans, show big individual differences in their responses to environmental stress. Some are very fearful and anxious in response to changes in their environment, others are aggressive and impulsive. One can identify genetic and environmental patterns that contribute to these differences, but recent evidence suggests that the interaction between genes and environment is of equal importance, possibly through mechanisms by which specific aspects of the environment influence the expression of certain genes at specific times of development [20].

The association between behavioral trait measures collected early in life and later social adaptation and personality in adult non-human primates is only beginning to be understood [20,91-94]. In humans, behavioral disinhibition measured early in life is a known predictor of impulsivity and aggression in adolescence and adulthood [95,96]. If this is also true for non-human primates, 5-HTT plays a role in the “cycle of violence” phenomenon observed in humans and rhesus macaques [49,97-99].

In this context, the animal model that helps us to understand self-harm is the Diathesis-Stress Model. We take rhesus macaques, who share 95% of their genes with humans and who possess a similar adaptive capacity, as an example. Other species like chimpanzees that share more than 98% of their genes with humans have the monozygotic gene. So why did we choose rhesus macaques as our integrated model? Rhesus macaques have short and long alleles of the serotonin transporter gene like humans; they share with humans the same biodiversity. In both of them, the childhood environment plus the individual genetic determinates the level of violence in later life coping with stress. In this sense, although humans and chimpanzees share more genes, rhesus monkeys and humans possess a bigger similarity: the biodiversity in the individual behavior regarding impulsivity and aggression. This peculiarity is what makes them more versatile in their adaptation capacity.

Having a negative emotional experience with caregivers during childhood precipitates the development of an abnormal mental model about relations, which continues manifesting during adulthood. Relationship with early caregivers decisively influences the way adults maintain relations afterwards. If these first contacts are insufficient or anomalous in some way, there will be repercussions in later social life [48].

Furthermore, early environments may play an important role through their influence on psychobiological functions in adulthood [100,101]. Contributing to the information about the kinds of environment, the organism will have to be dealt with in the future. In addition, there are several studies that highlight the correlation between the serotonin transporter gene and stress [104- 109]. Again, recent work has succeeded in delineating some of the specific psychobiological factors that underlie such programming, including both genetic and epigenetic mechanisms [102,103].

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Stefanie Zauner for helping with the translation.

REFERENCES

  1. B. G. Tate and G. S. Baroff, “Aversive Control of SelfInjurious Behaviour in a Psychotic Boy,” Behaviour Research and Therapy, Vol. 4, 1966, pp. 281-287.
  2. M. D. Davenport, C. K. Lutz, S. Tiefenbacher, M. A. Novak and J. S. Meyer, “A Rhesus Monkey Model of Self-Injury: Effects of Relocation Stress on Behaviour and Neuroendocrine Function,” Biological Psychiatry, Vol. 63, No. 10, 2008, pp. 990-996. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.10.025
  3. C. Lutz, A. Well and M. Novak, “Stereotypic and SelfInjurious Behaviour in Rhesus Macaques: A Survey and Retrospective Analysis of Environment and Early Experience,” American Journal of Primatology, Vol. 60, No. 1, 2003, pp. 1-15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.10075
  4. J. Haines, C. L. Williams, K. L. Brain and G. V. Wilson, “The Psychophysiology of Self-Mutilation,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Vol. 104, No. 3,1995, pp. 471-489. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.104.3.471
  5. S. Herpertz, “Self-Injurious Behaviour. Psychopathological and Nosological Characteristics in Subtypes of Self- Injurers,” Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, Vol. 91, No. 1, 1995, pp. 57-68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1995.tb09743.x
  6. N. Huband and D. Tantam, “Repeated Self-Wounding: Women’s Recollection of Pathways to Cutting and of the Value of Different Interventions,” Psychology and Psychotherapy, Vol. 77, No. Pt 4, 2004, pp. 413-428. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/1476083042555370
  7. M. M. Linehan, “Dialectical Behavior Therapy for Treatment of Borderline Personality Disorder: Implications for the Treatment of Substance Abuse,” NIDA Research Monograph, Vol. 137, 1993, pp. 201-216.
  8. M. K. Nixon, P. F. Cloutier and S. Aggarwal, “Affect Regulation and Addictive Aspects of Repetitive Self-Injury in Hospitalized Adolescents,” Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Vol. 41, No. 11, 2002, pp. 1333-1341. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200211000-00015
  9. J. Whitlock, J. Eckenrode and D. Silverman, “Self-Injurious Behaviors in a College Population,” Pediatrics, Vol. 117, No. 6, 2006, pp. 1939-1948. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-2543
  10. J. Briere and E. Gil, “Self-Mutilation in Clinical and General Population Samples: Prevalence, Correlates, and Functions,” American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, Vol. 68, No. 4, 1998, pp. 609-620. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0080369
  11. F. J. Symons, K. A. Sutton, C. Walker and J. W. Bodfish, “Altered Diurnal Pattern of Salivary Substance P in Adults with Developmental Disabilities and Chronic Self-Injury,” American Journal on Mental Retardation, Vol. 108, No. 1, 2003, pp. 13-18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1352/0895-8017(2003)108<0013:ADPOSS>2.0.CO;2
  12. K. A. L. Bayne and M. A. Novak, “Psychological Disorders,” In: B. T. Bennett, C. R. Abee and R. Henrickson, Eds., Nonhuman Primates in Biomedical Research, Vol. II, Diseases, Academic Press, New York, 1998, pp. 485- 500.
  13. M. A. Novak, C. M. Crockett and G. P. Sackett, “Self-Injurious Behaviour in Captive Macaque Monkeys,” In: S. R. Schroeder, M. Oster-Granite and T. Thompson, Eds., Self Injurious Behavior: Gene-Brain-Behavior Relationships, American Psychological Association Press, Washington DC, 2002, pp. 151-162. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10457-010
  14. S. Tiefenbacher, M. A. Novak, C. K. Lutz and J. S. Meyer, “The Physiology and Neurochemistry of Self-Injurious Behaviour: A Non-Human Primate Model,” Frontiers in Bioscience, Vol. 10, No. 1, 2005, pp. 1-11. http://dx.doi.org/10.2741/1500
  15. A. C. Wilson, S. S. Carlson and T. J. White, “Biochemical Evolution,” Annual Review of Biochemistry, Vol. 46, No. 1, 1977, pp. 573-639. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.bi.46.070177.003041
  16. J. M. Gorman, S. Mathew and J. Coplan, “Neurobiology of Early Life Stress: Nonhuman Primate Models,” Seminars in Clinical Neuropsychiatry, Vol. 7, No. 2, 2002, pp. 96-103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1053/scnp.2002.31784
  17. M. M. Sanchez, C. O. Ladd and P. M. Plotsky, “Early Adverse Experience as a Developmental Risk Factor for Later Psychopathology: Evidence from Rodent and Primate Models,” Development and Psychopathology, Vol. 13, No. 3, 2001, pp. 419-449. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579401003029
  18. C. S. Barr, T. K. Newman, M. L. Becker, et al., “Early Experience and rh5-HTTPLR Genotype Interact to Influence Social Behaviour and Aggression in Nonhuman Primates,” Genes, Brain Behavior, 2003, in press.
  19. C. S. Barr, M. L. Schwandt, T. K. Newman and J. D. Higley, “The Use of Adolescent Nonhuman Primates to Model Human Alcohol Intake: Neurobiological, Genetic, and Psychological Variables,” Annals of the New York Academy of Science, Vol. 1021, 2004, pp. 221-233. http://dx.doi.org/10.1196/annals.1308.027
  20. S. J. Suomi, “Risk, Resilience, and Gene X Environment Interactions in Rhesus Monkeys,” Annals of New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 1994, 2006, pp. 52-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1196/annals.1376.006
  21. J. Wendland, et al., “Structural Variation of the Monamine Oxidase A Gene Promoter Repeat Polymorphism in Nonhuman Primates,” Genes, Brain and Behavior, Vol. 5, No. 1, 2005, pp. 40-45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00130.x
  22. A. F. Richard, F. J. Goldstein and R. E. Dewar, “Weed Macaques: The Evolutionary Implications of Macaque Feeding Ecology,” International Journal of Primatology, Vol. 10, No. 6, 1989, pp. 569-594. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02739365
  23. J. K. Buitelaar, “Self-Injurious Behaviour in Retarded Children: Clinical Phenomena and Biological Mechanisms,” Acta Paedopsychiatrica, Vol. 56, 1993, pp. 105- 111.
  24. R. M. Winchel and M. Stanley, “Self-Injurious Behavior: A Review of the Behavior and Biology of Self-Mutilation,” American Journal of Psychiatry, Vol. 148, 1991, pp. 306-317.
  25. A. S. New, R. L. Trestman, V. Mitropoulou, D. S. Benishay, E. Coccaro, J. Silverman and L. J. Siever, “Serotonergic Function and Self-Injurious Behavior in Personality Disorder Patients,” Psychiatry Research, Vol. 69, No. 1, 1997, pp. 17-26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(96)02784-9
  26. B. H. Herman, “A Possible Role of Proopiomelanocortin Peptides in Self-Injurious Behavior,” Progress in NeuroPsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, Vol. 14, No. S1, 1990, pp. S109-S139. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0278-5846(90)90091-T
  27. G. R. Breese, H. E. Criswell, G. E. Duncan and R. A. Mueller, “Dopamine Deficiency in Self-Injurious Behavior,” Psychopharmacology Bulletin, Vol. 25, 1989, pp. 353-357.
  28. M. Goldstein, “Dopaminergic Mechanisms in Self-Inflicting Biting Behavior,” Psychopharmacology Bulletin, Vol. 25, 1989, pp. 349-352.
  29. S. P. Sivam, “GBR-12909-Induced Self-Injurious Behavior: Role of Dopamine,” Brain Research, Vol. 690, No. 2, 1995, pp. 259-263. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(95)00604-O
  30. S. P. Sivam, “Dopamine, Serotonin and Tachykinin in Self-Injurious Behavior,” Life Science, Vol. 58, No. 26, 1996, pp. 2367-2375. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0024-3205(96)00121-X
  31. R. S. Chamberlain and B. H. Herman, “A Novel Biochemical Model Linking Dysfunctions in Brain Melatonin, Proopiomelanocortin Peptides, and Serotonin in Autism,” Biological Psychiatry, Vol. 28, No. 9, 1990, pp. 773-793. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-3223(90)90513-2
  32. C. A. Sandman, “Beta-Endorphin Disregulation in Autistic and Self-Injurious Behavior: A Neurodevelopmental Hypothesis,” Synapse, Vol. 2, No. 3, 1988, pp. 193-199. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/syn.890020304
  33. S. Tiefenbacher, M. A. Novak, L. M. Marinus, W. K. Chase, J. A. Miller and J. S. Meyer, “Altered Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenocortical Function in Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) with Self-Injurious Behaviour,” Psychoneuroendocrinology, Vol. 29, No. 4, 2004, pp. 501-515. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00068-4
  34. C. A. Sandman and W. P. Hetrick, “Opiate Mechanisms in Self-Injury,” Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, Vol. 1, No. 2, 1995, pp. 130-136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.1410010209
  35. C. A. Sandman and P. Touchette, “Opioids and the Maintenance of Self-Injurious Behavior,” In: S. R. Schroeder, M. L. Oster-Granite and T. Thompson, Eds., Self-Injurious Behavior: Gene-Brain-Behavior Relationships, American Psychological Association, Washington DC, 2002, pp. 191-204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10457-013
  36. T. Thompson, “Self-Injurious Behavior as Endogenous Neurochemical Self-Administration,” Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, Vol. 1, No. 2, 1995, pp. 137-148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.1410010210
  37. M. F. Cataldo and J. C. Harris, “The Biological Basis for Self-Injury in the Mentally Retarded,” Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, Vol. 2, No. 1, 1982, pp. 21-39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0270-4684(82)90004-0
  38. L. M. Marinus, W. K. Chase and M. A. Novak, “SelfBiting Behavior in Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta) Is Preferentially Directed to Body Areas Associated with Acupuncture Analgesia,” American Journal of Primatology, Vol. 51, Suppl. 1, 2000, pp. 71-72.
  39. A. Gutierrez-García and C. Contreras, “El Suicidio y Algunos de Sus Correlatos Neurobiológicos. Primera Parte,” Salud Mental, Vol. 31, No. 4, 2008, pp. 321-330.
  40. A. Caspi, K. Sugden, T. E. Moffitt, et al., “Influences of Life Stress on Depression: Moderation by a Polymorphism in the 5-HTT Gene,” Science, Vol. 301, No. 5631, 2003, pp. 386-389. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1083968
  41. K. K. Watson, J. H. Ghodasra and M. L. Platt, “Serotonin Transporter Genotype Modulates Social Reward and Punishment in Rhesus Macaques,” PLoS ONE, Vol. 4, No. 1, 2009, Article ID: e4156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004156
  42. K. P. Lesch, J. Meyer, K. Glatz, et al., “The 5-HT Transporter Gene-Linked Polymorphic Region (5-HTTPR) in Evolutionary Perspective: Alternative Biallelic Variation in Rhesus Monkeys,” Journal of Neural Transmission, Vol. 104, No. 11-12, 1997, pp. 1259-1266. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01294726
  43. J. D. Higley, W. W. Thompson, M. Champoux, D. Goldman, M. F. Hasert, G. W. Kraemer, J. M. Scanlan, S. J. Suomi and M. Linnoila, “Paternal and Maternal Genetic and Environmental Contributions to CSF Monoamine Metabolites in Thesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta),” JAMA Psychiatry, Vol. 50, No. 8, 1993, pp. 615-623. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820200025003
  44. S. J. Suomi, “Gene-Environment Interactions and the Neurobiology of Social Conflict,” Annals of New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 1008, 2003, pp. 132-139. http://dx.doi.org/10.1196/annals.1301.014
  45. A. Heils, A. Teufel, S. Petri, G. Stöber, P. Riederer, D. Bengel and K. P. Lesch, “Allelic Variation of Human Serotonin Transporter Gene Expression,” Journal of Neurochemistry, Vol. 66, No. 6, 1996, pp. 2621-2624.
  46. M. E. Wilson and B. Kinkead, “Gene-Environment Interactions, Not Neonatal Growth Hormone Deficiency, Time Puberty in Female Rhesus Monkeys,” Biology of Reproduction, Vol. 78, No. 4, 2008, pp. 736-743. http://dx.doi.org/10.1095/biolreprod.107.065953
  47. V. Michopoulos, S. L. Berga, J. R. Kaplan and M. E. Wilson, “Social Subordination and Polymorphisms in the Gene Encoding the Serotonin Transporter Enhance Estradiol Inhibition of Luteinizing Hormone Secretion in Female Rhesus Monkeys,” Biology of Reproduction, Vol. 81, No. 6, 2009, pp. 1154-1163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1095/biolreprod.109.079038
  48. J. D. Molina, F. López-Muñoz, D. J. Stein, M. J. MartínVázquez, C. Alamo, I. Lerma-Carrillo, C. Andrade-Rosa, M. V. Sánchez-López and M. de la Calle-Real, “Borderline Personality Disorder: A Review and Reformulation from Evolutionary Theory,” Medical Hypothesis, Vol. 73, No. 3, 2009, pp. 382-396. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2009.03.024
  49. E. L. Kinnally, E. R. Tarara, W. A. Mason, S. P. Mendoza, K. Abel, L. A. Lyons and J. P. Capitanio, “Serotonin Transporter Expression Is Predicted by Early Life Stress and Is Associated with Disinhibited Behavior in Infant Rhesus Macaques,” Genes, Brain and Behavior, Vol. 9, No. 1, 2010, pp. 45-52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-183X.2009.00533.x
  50. M. Ichise, D. Vines, T. Gura, G. Anderson, S. Suomi, J. Higley and R. B. Innis, “Effects of Early Life Stress on [11C]DASB Positron Emission Tomography Imaging of Serotonin Transporters in Adolescent Peerand MotherReared Rhesus Monkeys,” Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 26, No. 17, 2006, pp. 4638-4643. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5199-05.2006
  51. J. Jahng, J. Kim, H. Kim, B. Kim, D. Kang and J. Lee, “Chronic Food Restriction in Young Rats Results in Depressionand Anxiety-Like Behaviors with Decreased Expression of Serotonin Reuptake Transporter,” Brain Research, Vol. 1150, 2007, pp. 100-107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.080
  52. E. Kinnally, L. Lyons, K. Abel, S. Mendoza and J. P. Capitanio, “Effects of Early Experience and Genotype on Serotonin Transporter Regulation in Infant Rhesus Macaques,” Genes, Brain and Behavior, Vol. 7, No. 4, 2008, pp. 481-486. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00383.x
  53. J. H. Lee, H. J. Kim, J. G. Kim, V. Ryu, B. T. Kim, D. W. Kang and J. W. Jahng, “Depressive Behaviors and Decreased Expression of Serotonin Reuptake Transporter in Rats that Experienced Neonatal Maternal Separation,” Neuroscience Research, Vol. 58, No. 1, 2007, pp. 32-39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2007.01.008
  54. J. Miller, E. Kinnally, R. Ogden, M. Oquendo, J. Mann and R. Parsey, “Reported Childhood Abuse Is Associated with Low Serotonin Transporter Binding in Vivo in Major Depressive Disorder,” Synapse, Vol. 63, No. 7, 2009, pp. 565-573. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/syn.20637
  55. C. S. Barr, T. K. Newman, M. L. Becker, M. Champoux, K. P. Lesch, S. J. Suomi, D. Goldman and J. Dee Higley, “Serotonin Transporter Gene Variation Is Associated with Alcohol Sensitivity in Rhesus Macaques Exposed to EarlyLife Stress,” Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, Vol. 27, No. 5, 2003, pp. 812-817. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.ALC.0000067976.62827.ED
  56. C. S. Barr, T. K Newman, C. Shannon, et al., “Rearing Condition and rh5-HTTLPR Interact to Influence LHPAAxis Response to Stress in Infant Macaques,” Biological Psychiatry, Vol. 55, No. 7, 2004, pp. 733-738. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.12.008
  57. A. J. Bennett, K. P. Lesch, A. Heils, J. C. Long, J. G. Lorenz, S. E. Shoaf, M. Champoux, S. J. Suomi, M. V. Linnoila and J. D. Higley, “Early Experience and Serotonin Transporter Gene Variation Interact to Influence Primate CNS Function,” Molecular Psychiatry, Vol. 7, No. 1, 2002, pp. 118-122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.mp.4000949
  58. D. Gallardo-Pujol, C. G. Forero, A. Maydeu-Olivares and A. Andrés-Pueyo, “Desarrollo del Comportamiento Antisocial: Factores Psicobiológicos, Ambientales e Interacciones Genotipo-Ambiente,” Revista Neurología, Vol. 48, No. 4, 2009, pp. 191-198.
  59. A. J. Bennett, A. Heils, K. P. Lesch and M. Linnoila, “Serotonin Transporter Gene Variation, CSF 5-HIAA Concentrations, and Alcohol-Related Aggression in Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta),” American Journal of Primatology, Vol. 45, No. 2, 1998, pp. 168-169.
  60. C. S. Barr, T. K. Newman, M. L. Becker, C. C. Parker, M. Champoux, K. P. Lesch, D. Goldman, S. J. Suomi and J. D. Higley, “The Utility of the Non-Human Primate: Model for Studying Gene by Environment Interactions in Behavioral Research,” Genes, Brain and Behavior, Vol. 2, No. 6, 2003, pp. 336-340. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1601-1848.2003.00051.x
  61. M. Champoux, A. Bennett, C. Shannon, J. D. Higley, K. P. Lesch and S. J. Suomi, “Serotonin Transporter Gene Polymorphism, Differential Early Rearing, and Behavior in Rhesus Monkey Neonates,” Molecular Psychiatry, Vol. 7, No. 10, 2002, pp. 1058-1063. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.mp.4001157
  62. E. L. Kinnally, G. M. Karere, L. A. Lyons, S. P. Mendoza, W. A. Mason and J. P. Capitanio, “Serotonin Pathway Gene-Gene and Gene-Environment Interactions Influence Behavioural Stress Response in Infant Rhesus Macaques,” Development and Psychopathology, Vol. 22, No. 1, 2010, pp. 35-44.
  63. M. A. Novak, “Self-Injurious Behavior in Rhesus Monkeys: New Insights into Its Etiology, Physiology, and Treatment,” American Journal of Primatology, Vol. 59, No. 1, 2003, pp. 3-19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.10063
  64. M. J. Jorgensen, J. H. Kinsey and M. A. Novak, “Risk Factors for Self-Injurious Behavior in Captive Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta),” American Journal of Primatology, Vol. 45, 1998, p. 187.
  65. C. K. Lutz, W. K. Chase and M. A. Novak, “Abnormal Behavior in Singly-Housed Macaca mulatta: Prevalence and Potential Risk Factors,” American Journal of Primatology, Vol. 51, Suppl. 1, 2000, p. 71.
  66. R. U. Bellanca and C. M. Crockett, “Factors Predicting Increased Incidence of Abnormal Behavior in Male Pigtailed Macaques,” American Journal of Primatology, Vol. 58, No. 2, 2002, pp. 57-69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.10052
  67. C. K. Lutz and M. A. Novak, “Environmental Enrichment for Nonhuman Primates: Theory and Application,” ILAR Journal, Vol. 46, No. 2, 2005, pp. 178-191. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ilar.46.2.178
  68. M. A. Novak and A. J. Petto, “Perspectives on Psychological Well-Being in Captive Primates: Through the Looking Glass,” In: M. A. Novak and A. J. Petto, Eds., Through the Looking Glass: Issues of Psychological WellBeing in Captive Nonhuman Primates, American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, 1991, pp. 1-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10080-023
  69. M. A. Novak and S. J. Suomi, “Psychological Well-Being of Primates in Captivity,” American Psychologist, Vol. 43, No. 10, 1988, pp. 765-773. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.43.10.765
  70. M. A. Novak and G. P. Sackett, “The Effects of Rearing Experiences: The Early Years,” In: G. P. Sackett, G. C. Ruppenthal and K. Elias, Eds., Nursery Rearing of Nonhuman Primates in the 21st Century, Springer, Chicago, 2006, pp. 5-19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-25640-5_1
  71. I. Rommeck, K. Anderson, A. Heagerty, A. Cameron and B. McCowan, “Risk Factors and Remediation of SelfInjurious and Self-Abuse Behaviour in Rhesus Macaques,” Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, Vol. 12, No. 1, 2009, pp. 61-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888700802536798
  72. K. Baker, “Rearing and Housing History of Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta) Displaying Self-Injurious and Noninjurious Abnormal Behaviors,” American Journal of Primatology, Vol. 57, Suppl. 1, 2002, p. 82.
  73. H. Harlow and M. Harlow, “The Effect of Rearing Conditions on Behavior,” Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, Vol. 26, 1962, pp. 213-224.
  74. H. Harlow and M. Harlow, “Social Deprivation in Monkeys,” Scientific American, Vol. 207, 1962, pp. 136-146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican1162-136
  75. G. Berkson and W. Mason, “Stereotyped Behaviors of Chimpanzees: Relation to General Arousal and Alternative Activities,” Perceptual and Motor Skills, Vol. 19, No. 2, 1964, pp. 635-652. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1964.19.2.635
  76. C. Levison, “The Development of Head-Banging in a Young Rhesus Monkey,” American Journal of Mental Deficiency, Vol. 75, No. 3, 1970, pp. 323-328.
  77. H. J. McGrogan and J. E. King, “Repeated Separations of 2-Year-Old Squirrel Monkeys from Familiar Mother Surrogates,” American Journal of Primatology, Vol. 3, No. 1-4, 1982, pp. 285-290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350030126
  78. E. D. Klonsky, T. F. Oltmanns and E. Turkheimer, “Deliberate Self-Harm in a Nonclinical Population: Prevalence and Psychological Correlates,” American Journal of Primatology, Vol. 160, No. 8, 2003, pp. 1501-1508. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.8.1501
  79. J. V. Penn, C. L. Esposito, L. E. Schaeffer, G. K. Fritz and A. Spirito, “Suicide Attempts and Self-Mutilative Behaviour in a Juvenile Correctional Facility,” Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Vol. 42, No. 7, 2003, pp. 762-769. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.CHI.0000046869.56865.46
  80. S. Ross and N. L. Heath, “Two Models of Adolescent Self-Mutilation,” Suicide & Life-Threatening Behavior, Vol. 33, No. 3, 2003, pp. 277-287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/suli.33.3.277.23218
  81. S. Tiefenbacher, M. A. Fahey, A. L. Pouliot, B. M. Kaufman, J. K. Rowlett and M. A. Novak, “Effects of Diazepam Treatment on the Incidence of Self-Injurious Behavior in Individually Housed Male Rhesus Monkeys,” American Journal of Primatology, Vol. 62, 2004, p. 112.
  82. S. L. Watson, J. G. McCoy, R. C. Stavisky, T. F. Greer and D. Hanbury, “Cortisol Responses to Relocation Stress in Garnett’s Bushbaby (Otolemur garnetti),” Contemporary Topics, Vol. 44, No. 3, 2005, pp. 22-24.
  83. S. K. Lutgendorf, T. T. Reimer, J. H. Harvey, G. Marks, S. Y. Hong, S. L. Hillis and D. M. Lubaroff, “Effects of Housing Relocation on Immunocompetence and Psychosocial Functioning in Older Adults,” The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, Vol. 56, No. 2, 2001, pp. M97-M105.
  84. S. J. Suomi, “Primate Separation Models of Affective Disorders,” In: J. Madden, Ed., Neurobiology of Learning, Emotion, and Affect, Raven, New York, 1991, pp. 195- 214.
  85. J. S. Scanlan, “Continuity of Stress Responsivity in Infant Monkeys (Macaca mulatta): State, Hormonal, Dominance, and Genetic Status,” Ph.D. Thesis, University of Wisconsin, Madison, 1988.
  86. D. E. Williamson, et al., “Heritability of Fearful-Anxious Endophenotypes in Infant Rhesus Macaques: A Preliminary Report,” Biological Psychiatry, Vol. 53, No. 4, 2003, pp. 284-291. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01601-3
  87. S. J. Suomi, “Attachment in Rhesus Monkeys,” In: J. Cassidy and P. Shaver, Eds., Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, Guilford, New York, 1999, pp. 181- 197.
  88. J. D. Higley, S. J. Suomi and M. Linoilla, “A Longitudinal Assessment of CSF Monoamine Metabolites and Plasma Cortisol Concentrations in Young Rhesus Monkeys,” Biological Psychiatry, Vol. 32, No. 2, 1992, pp. 127-145.
  89. C. Shannon, M. L. Schwandt, M. Champoux, S. E. Shoaf, S. J. Suomi, M. Linnoila and J. D. Higley, “Maternal Absence and Stability of Individual Differences in CSF 5-HIAA Concentrations in Rhesus Monkey Infants,” American Journal of Psychiatry, Vol. 162, No. 9, 2005, pp. 1658-1666.
  90. B. Kolb, R. Gibb and T. E. Robinson, “Brain Plasticity and Behavior,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, Vol. 12, No. 1, 2003, pp. 1-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.01210
  91. L. Fairbanks, M. Jorgensen, A. Huff, K. Blau, Y. Y. Hung and J. J. Mann, “Adolescent Impulsivity Predicts Adult Dominance Attainment in Male Vervet Monkeys,” American Journal of Primatology, Vol. 64, No. 1, 2004, pp. 1-17.
  92. E. C. Sullivan and J. P. Capitanio, “Consistency in Behavioral Responsiveness from Infancy to Adulthood,” Paper presented at the American Society for Primatologists, West Palm Beach, 2008.
  93. T. Weinstein and J. Capitanio, “Individual Differences in Infant Temperament Predict Social Relationships of Yearling Rhesus Monkeys, Macaca mulatta,” Animal Behaviour, Vol. 76, No. 2, 2008, pp. 455-460.
  94. K. J. Hinde and J. P. Capitanio, “Lactational Programming? Mother’s Milk Energy Predicts Infant Behavior and Temperament in Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta),” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 2009, in press.
  95. A. Caspi and P. Silva, “Temperamental Qualities at Age Three Predict Personality Traits in Young Adulthood: Longitudinal Evidence from a Birth Cohort,” Child Development, Vol. 66, No. 2, 1995, pp. 486-498.
  96. S. Olson, J. Bates, J. Sandy and E. Schilling, “Early Developmental Precursors of Impulsive and Inattentive Behavior: From Infancy to Middle Childhood,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Vol. 43, No. 4, 2002, pp. 435-447.
  97. A. Caspi, J. McClay, T. Moffitt, J. Mill, J. Martin, I. Craig, A. Taylor and R. Poulton, “Role of Genotype in the Cycle of Violence in Maltreated Children,” Science, Vol. 297, No. 5582, 2002, pp. 851-854.
  98. K. Dodge, J. Bates and G. S. Pettit, “Mechanisms in the Cycle of Violence,” Science, Vol. 250, No. 4988, 1990, pp. 1678-1683.
  99. D. Maestripieri, J. D. Higley, S. Lindell, T. Newman, K. McCormack and M. Sanchez, “Early Maternal Rejection Affects the Development of Monoaminergic Systems and Adult Abusive Parenting in Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta),” Behavioral Neuroscience, Vol. 120, No. 5, 2006, pp. 1017-1024.
  100. P. D. Gluckman and M. A. Hanson, “Living with the Past: Evolution, Development, and Patterns of Disease,” Science, Vol. 305, No. 5691, 2004, pp. 1733-1736.
  101. D. Koubovec, L. Geerts, H. J. Odendaal, D. J. Stein and B. Vythilingum, “Effects of Psychologic Stress on Fetal Development and Pregnancy Outcome,” Current Psychiatry Reports, Vol. 7, No. 4, 2005, pp. 274-280. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11920-005-0081-9
  102. L. R. Starr, C. Hammen, P. A. Brennan and J. M. Najman, “Serotonin Transporter Gene as a Predictor of Stress Generation in Depression,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Vol. 121, No. 4, 2012, pp. 810-818.
  103. A. Mueller, D. Armbruster, D. A. Moser, T. Canli, K. P. Lesch, B. Brocke and C. Kirschbaum, “Interaction of Serotonin Transporter Gene-Linked Polymorphic Region and Stressful Life Events Predicts Cortisol Stress Response,” Neuropsychopharmacology, Vol. 36, No. 7, 2011, pp. 1332-1339.
  104. R. A. Morey, A. R. Hariri, A. L. Gold, M. A. Hauser, H. J. Munger, F. Dolcos and G. McCarthy, “Serotonin Transporter Gene Polymorphisms and Brain Function during Emotional Distraction from Cognitive Processing in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder,” BMC Psychiatry, Vol. 11, 2011, p. 76.
  105. K. Karg, M. Burmeister, K. Shedden and S. Sen, “The Serotonin Transporter Promoter Variant (5-HTTLPR), Stress, and Depression Meta-Analysis Revisited: Evidence of Genetic Moderation,” Archives of General Psychiatry, Vol. 68, No. 5, 2011, pp. 444-454.
  106. M. A. C. Stephens, M. E. McCaul, E. M. Weerts and G. Varita, “Serotonin Transporter-Linked Polymorphic Region (5-HTTLPR) Genotype Is Associated with Cortisol Responsivity to Naloxone Challenger,” Psychopharmacology, Vol. 224, No. 2, 2012, pp. 223-230.
  107. A. J. Jasinska, C. A. Lowry and M. Burmeister, “Serotonin Transporter Gene, Stress and Raphe-Raphe Interactions: A Molecular Mechanism of Depression,” Trends in Neurosciences, Vol. 35, No. 7, 2012, pp. 454-455. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2012.03.002
  108. I. C. G. Weaver, N. Cervoni, F. A. Champagne, A. C. D’Alessio, S. Sharma and J. R. Seckl, “Epigenetic Programming by Maternal Behavior,” Nature Neuroscience, Vol. 7, 2004, pp. 847-854.
  109. M. J. Meaney and M. Szyf, “Environmental Programming of Stress Responses through DNA Methylation: Life at the Interface between a Dynamic Environment and a Fixed Genome,” Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, Vol. 7, No. 2, 2005, pp. 103-123.

NOTES

*Corresponding author.