Open Journal of Social Sciences
2013. Vol.1, No.6, 81-86
Published Online November 2013 in SciRes (http://www.scirp.org/journal/jss) http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/jss.2013.16013
Open Access 81
Psychosocial Risk Factors of Cancer Diseases: How Specific
Are They?
Shulamith Kreitler1, Michal M. Kreitler1, Frida Barak2
1Departme nt of Psychology, T el Aviv University and Psychooncology Research Center,
Sheba Medical Center, Tel Hashomer, Israel
2Oncology Department, Barzilai Medical Center, Ashkelon, Israel
Email: krit@netvision.net.il
Received 2013
The paper presents the psychological correlates of colorectal cancer patients in the framework of the cog-
nitive orientation (CO) theory. According to this cognitive-motivational approach the major factors re-
sponsible for disease are a pathogene, background factors and a physiological program implementing the
disease processes. Psychological factors form a part of the background factors. They consist of specific
cognitive contents forming a motivational disposition, representing four types of beliefs referring to spe-
cific contents. The cognitive contents are assessed by a CO questionnaire. Previous studies showed that
the CO questionnaire differentiated significantly between colorectal cancer patients and healthy controls
in two samples, as well as between both groups and Crohn patients who have potential malignancy. The
present study was designed to examine the specificity of the psychological variables characterizing colo-
rectal cancer. The CO questionnaire was administered to male colorectal cancer patients and patients with
prostate cancer and male healthy controls, and to female colorectal cancer patients, patients with breast
cancer and healthy female controls. In both cases the scores of the CO questionnaire differentiated be-
tween the colorectal cancer patients and the two other groups, but there were no differences between the
prostate cancer patients or the breast cancer patients and the healthy controls. These findings confirm the
hypothesis that the CO variables of colorectal cancer are disease specific and may be considered as psy-
chological risk factors for colorectal cancer.
Keywords: Colorectal Cancer; Personality; Cognitive Orientation; Risk Factors
Introduction
Search for risk factors of cancer diseases in the Medline file
yields 81,380 items (for Dec. 3, 2013), listing variables such as
genetics, age, nutrition, obesity, ethnic background, alcohol
consumption, lack of physical exercise, or ingestion of specific
medications but very few if any refer to psychosocial risk fac-
tors. The paucity of scientific interest in psychological risk
factors of cancer is not surprising in view of controversial find-
ings in this domain. Some of the findings, such as about the
Type C personality type (Temoshok, Heller, Sagebiel et al.,
1985) were not reproduced (Amelang, 1997; Fox, 1998; Good-
kin, Antoni, Sevin et al., 1993; Lilja, Smith, Malmstroem et al.,
1998; Servaes, Vingerhoets, Vreugdenhil et al., 1999), others
were shown to result from the disease itself (Kreitler, Kreitler,
& Chaitchik, 1993) and still others were identified as correlates
of chronic diseases in general (Denollet, 1998; Sanderman &
Ranchor, 1997).
The major shortcomings of the previous studies were that
they used subjects varying in medical and psychosocial charac-
teristics which could seriously affect the results, such as cancer
diagnoses, duration of disease, and quality of life; that they
mostly did not consider sufficiently relevant medical variables,
such as precise diagnosis, stage of disease, or medical risk fac-
tors; that the psychological variables selected for the studies
were not grounded in a theory that bound them to the disease
and sometimes could even be affected by the disease itself,
such as depression.
The present study is based on applying to the issue of psy-
chosocial risk factors for cancer diseases the theory of c ogn i t i ve
orientation (CO).
The Cognitive Ori entation Theory: The CO
Model of Health
The CO is a comprehensive cognitive-motivational theory of
a variety of outputs (Kreitler, 2004; Kreitler & Kreitler, 1976,
1982). A major version of the theory is the CO model of health,
which provides a well-validated and empirically-based ap-
proach to the impact of psychological variables on disease-
relevant physiological processes (Kreitler & Kreitler, 1991a,
1991b, 1996, 1998; Kreitler, 2003). The CO approach to phy-
sical diseases is based on the assumption that three major fac-
tors are necessary for the occurrence of a disease: the pathogene
which is the carrier or instigator of the disease, such as a virus
or a microbe; background factors that modulate the vulnerabil-
ity of the organism to succumb to the pathogene or resist its
impact; and a physiological mechanism or procedure that im-
plement the development and occurrence of the disease. The
CO theory focuses on the background factors. These may in-
clude intra-organismic variables, such as immunological state,
nutritional variables and co-morbidity, as well as extra-orga-
nismic variables, such as air pollution, toxins, and environmen-
tal danger. According to the CO theory, the background factors
include always also psychological factors. This however in no
way justifies considering the disease as a psychosomatic for the
S. KREITLER ET AL.
Open Access
82
background factors merely modulate the impact of the patho-
gene on the organism. The major advantage of the CO theory
for the study of psychological factors that play a predisposing
role in regard to cancer is that it provides a methodology for
identifying the adequate variables. Adequacy of the variables
means on the level of theory that they are relevant for a parti-
cular disease; they constitute theoretically a homogeneous and
comprehensive set of factors; and they are not under the con-
scious or voluntary control of the participant. On the empirical
level ade quacy of the va riables means t hat they may be empi ri-
cally validated, i.e., they significantly differentiate between pa-
tients and controls and they are amenable for change in a well-
controlled clinic al inter vention.
It is a characteristic of background factors that they are dis-
ease-specific, so much so that background factors, which may
promote disease occurrence for one disease, may act as inhibit-
ing factors for another disease, as in the case of cholesterol in
regard to cardiological and malignant diseases. Hence, a con-
struct as ‘stressis too general to do justice to the specificity of
the relevant psychosocial background factors.
The CO theory of physical health focuses on identifying and
changing the relevant psychological risk factors for diseases.
The main tenet of the theory is that the psychological risk
factors of the disease are cognitive contents of a specific kind.
The cognitive contents express how the individual conceptua-
lizes or experiences external and internal states and objects. The
cognitions are characterized in two respects: formally and in
terms of contents. The formal characterization is in terms of
four types of beliefs: a) Beliefs about self, which express in-
formation about oneself, such as one’s habits, actions or feel-
ings (e.g., I often get angry, I was born in London); b)
Be-
The characterization of the cognitions in terms of contents is
based on a standard procedure of stepwise meaning probing
administered in pretests with subjects, some of whom have the
disorder and some who do not (Kreitler & Kreitler, 1991a). The
procedure consists in asking the subjects to express the gener-
ally ac cepted meaning of the disease and related keywords, and
then to express successively the personal meanings of the
communicated meanings, three times in a row. Thus, schemati-
cally, the subject is asked to communicate the common mean-
ing of the disease and then to communicate the meaning of
what he or she stated and thus twice more. The resulting mean-
ings on the final level are assessed for similarities by indepen-
dent judges. The meanings that recur in at least 50% of the
subjects with the disease and no more than in 10% of those
without the disease are accepted as themesexpressing the
contents of the cognitive orientation of the disease. These
themes are used for constructing the CO questionnaire of the
disease: for each theme four beliefs are phrased, one in each
belief type. For example, the beliefs referring to the theme of
perfectionism could be the following: In whatever I do I try to
achieve perfection(belief about self); If one tries hard
enough it is possible to attain perfection(general belief); One
should never try to achieve perfection(belief about norm,
reversed); “I would like not to try to achieve perfection(belief
about goals, reversed). The CO questionnaire consists of four
parts, each devoted to one of the types of beliefs. In each part
there are randomly ordered statements to which the subject is
requested to respond by checking one of the following response
alternatives, scored 1 to 4: very true, true, not trueand
not at all true(in the case of goal beliefs the alternatives were
want very much, want , “don’t want, “don’t want at all).
The scores are the means of the responses the subject gave to
each set of statements referring to the four belief types. These
four scores are the major ones, complemented by scores re-
presenting the different themes in the CO questionnaire.
The four diseases belief types referring to the themes specific
for a particular disease form a vector representing the motiva-
tional disposition for the disease. If all four belief types support
the disease (i.e., the scores are high) the chances for the occur-
rence of the disease i the presence of a pathogene are increased.
The CO model of health has been applied successfully in regard
to different diseases, including cancer (Drechsler, Bruner, &
Kreitler, 1987; Kreitler, Levavi, & Bornstein, 1996; Kreitler &
Kreitler, 1991a; Nurymberg, Kreitler, & Weissler, 1996).
The CO of Colo rect al Cancer
The present study focuses on the psychological risk factors
for colorectal cancer. The CO of colorectal cancer has been
identified and presented in two previous studies. The first study
(Figer, Kreitler, Kreitler et al., 2002) focused on identifying and
establishing the psychological risk factors for colorectal cancer.
The CO questionnaire included beliefs of the four types that
referred to 44 themes that had been extracted from interviews
conducted with pretest participants (15 colon cancer patients
and 15 healthy controls) according to a standard procedure
(Kreitler & Kreitler, 1991a). The final draft of the questionnaire
was obtained after the completion of standard psychometric
analyses and tests, and included a total of 181 items presented
in four parts referring to each of the four types of beliefs. The
reliability was satisfactory for each part and for the whole ques-
tionnaire.
A factor analysis of the themes in the CO questionnaire
yiel ded the following eight factors: a) attaining peace and quiet
in one’s environment through restraint and self-regulation; b)
fulfilling one’s duties and commitments; c) suppressing anger;
d) toughness in regard to oneself; e) fighting injustice; f) con-
trolling others; g) avoiding being controlled by others; and h)
pleasing others.
The CO questionnaire was administered to 106 patients who
had been diagnosed with colorectal cancer and 99 healthy con-
trols. The results showed that the patients and controls differed
significantly in the four belief types, and in seven of the eight
factors. In all variables, the patients scored higher than the con-
trols, as predicted by the CO theory. Discriminant analyses
showed that on the basis of the CO variables alone, the subjects
were classified correctly as patients or healthy in 77% to 86.3%
of the cases, which indicates an improvement of 27% to 36.3%
over the 50% of correct classification expected by chance. The
best predictors of the belief types were beliefs about self and
general beliefs, and of the factors the first factor (attaining
peace and quiet in one’s environment by self-regulation), the
liefs about rules and norms, which express ethical, esthetic,
social and other rules and standards (e.g., One should be asser-
tive”); c) General beliefs, which express information concern-
ing others and the environment (e.g., The world is a dangerous
place”; and d) Beliefs about goals, which express actions or
states desired or undesired by the individual (e.g., I want to be
respected by others). Formally, the beliefs differ in the subject
(in beliefs about self and goals the subject is the self, in general
beliefs and norms it is non-self) and in the relation between
subject and predicate (in beliefs about self and general beliefs it
is factual, in norms the desirable, in goals the desired).
S. KREITLER ET AL.
Open Access
83
fourth factor (toughness in regard to oneself) and the sixth fac-
tor 6 (controlling others). Since the scores of the CO variables
do not depend on demographic and medical characteristics,
such as disease duration, disease site and stages, quality of life
and age, the observed differences in the CO variables between
patients and healthy controls may be considered as characteriz-
ing the tested groups of participants per se.
The described study was the first to show that there exists a
set of psychological variables, defined on the basis of the CO
theory, which characterize colon cancer patients and differen-
tiate between them and healthy controls. These findings support
the hypothesis that there is a set of comprehensive, disease-
relevant, reliable, specific psychological variables that may be
considered as potential risk factors for colon cancer.
The findings were confirmed in a further study which
showed that the CO questionnaire of colorectal cancer differen-
tiated significantly not only between colorectal cancer patients
and healthy controls but also between these two groups and a
group of patients with Crohn’s disease which is a disea se with a
potential for malignancy (Kreitler, Kreitler, Len et al., 2008).
As expected, the scores of the latter patients were midway be-
tween those of the colorectal cancer patients and the healthy
controls. The prediction of group membership by discriminant
analysis was highly significant. In addition, the study identified
the thematic structure of the CO questionnaire of colorectal
cancer. The tendencies for compulsiveness, control of oneself
and especially of anger, self effacement, pleasing others, self
assertion, distancing oneself from others, keeping regulations,
and performing to perfection all ones obligations formed the
following three major foci: perfect duty performance, and two
contradictory pairs: self effacement versus self assertion, and
closeness to others versus distancing from others. The clusters
and the contrasts constitute potentially sources of tension.
The Present Study
Objectives
The purpose of the present study was to examine in depth the
claim of the specificity of the CO of colorectal cancer. As noted
above, specificity is a major characteristic required of CO-
defined risk factors for a disease. Up to now the only basis for
the claim of specificity of this CO is the method whereby the
cognitive contents constituting it have been derived. In line
with this method, the cognitive contents defining the themes of
the CO questionnaire are based on interviewing according to a
standard procedure individuals diagnosed with colorectal can-
cer and individuals without this disease. The specificity claim is
further enhanced by the evidence, that the CO questionnaire of
colorectal cancer differentiated significantly between patients
with colorectal cancer and healthy controls, as well as patients
with Crohn’s disease, who merely have a tendency for malig-
nancy. However, the procedure and the findings do not exclude
the possibility that the CO questionnaire of colorectal cancer
applies also to other types of cancer and differentiate between
them and individuals without cancer. If that were the case, the
CO questionnaire of colorectal cancer should be considered
perhaps as the CO questionnaire of cancer in general. In view
of this possibility, the purpose of the present study was to test
the specificity of the CO questionnaire of colorectal cancer by
applying it to two types of cancer other than colorectal cancer:
prostate cancer and breast cancer. Because of the involvement
of gender in the case of prostate and breast cancers, the groups
of colorectal cancer patients and healthy controls used for
comparison consisted also of one or the other gender. In this
manner the specificity claim could be put to the test in a broader
range of cancer diagnoses. Considering the specificity claim
grounded in the CO theory, the hypotheses were that the CO
questionnaire of colorectal cancer would differentiate between
colorectal cancer patients, on the one hand, and prostate cancer
patients or breast cancer patients as well as healthy controls, on
the other hand. Further, the CO questionnaire of colorectal
cancer would not differentiate significantly between prostate
cancer patients and healthy controls or breast cancer patients
and healthy controls. The rationale for these hypotheses is that
the CO questionnaire of colorectal cancer is a tool specifically
targeting the cognitive orientation of colorectal cancer.
Method
Participants
The participants in the study were 230 colorectal cancer pa-
tients (101 men and 129 women), whose mean age was 61.5 yrs,
disease duration .5 to 7.3 yrs, with different diagnoses (47%
cancer of the rectum, 53% colon cancer), and in different dis-
ease stages (I 13.04%, II 38.7%, III 30%, IV 18.26%). The
healthy controls included 165 individuals with no diagnosed
diseases. They included 55 men and 110 women. The controls
were matched to the patients in age and years of education.
They were recruited in offices of the hospital where the study
was conducted and in the university. Both of these groups in-
cluded subjects who have participated in previ ous studies (Figer,
Kreitler, & Chaitchik, 2002; Kreitler et al., 2008). Two addi-
tional groups participated in the study: 61 male patients with
prostate cancer and 53 female patients with breast cancer. Each
group included patients of the four stages of disease. The pros-
tate cancer patients and breast cancer patients were matched to
the colorectal cancer patients and healthy controls in age and
years of education. None of the participants in the three cancer
groups had co-morbidity or psychiatric disorders.
Tools
All participants were administered the CO questionnaire of
colorectal cancer, used in the previous studies (Figer, Kreitler,
& Chaitchik, 2002; Kreitler et al., 2008). The questionnaire
included four parts administered together, referring to beliefs
about self, general beliefs, beliefs about norms and beliefs
about goals. In each part there were statements (a total of 181)
referring to 44 themes, to which the subject was requested to
respond by selecting one of the four response alternatives (e.g.,
Very true, “True”, Not true, etc.). The questionnaire was
presented without any special introduction so that the respon-
dents did not know that it referred to colorectal cancer.
Procedure
The patient participants responded to the CO questionnaire
when they visited the hospital for treatment or follow-up, and
the healthy controls responded in their working places.
Results
The major variables used in the different analyses were the
mean scores of the four types of beliefs.
Table 1 shows the results of mean comparisons between the
S. KREITLER ET AL.
Open Access
84
Table 1.
Means and Sds and comparison of the scores of colorectal cancer pa-
tients and healthy controls in the four types of beliefs of the CO theory.
Belief Type Colorectal Cancer
Patient (N = 230) Healthy Controls
(N = 165)
Mean Sd Mean Sd
Beliefs about self 2.79 .34 2.52*** .26
Beliefs about norms 2.55 .39 2.28*** .31
General beliefs 2.69 .35 2.38*** .27
Beliefs about goals 2.75 .36 2.49*** .29
Note. ***The mean differences in the four belief types between colorectal cancer
patients and heal thy controls are significan t, p < .001.
group of colorectal cancer patients and the healthy controls,
without differentiating between men and women. This table
was designed to examine the validity of the CO questionnaire
of colorectal cancer by test anew for differentiating between
colorectal cancer patients and healthy controls. The results
show that in all four mean scores of the four belief types the
differences between colorectal cancer patients and the healthy
controls were highly signi ficant, with the patients scoring higher,
as expected in line with the CO theory.
Table 2 presents the results of comparing the mean scores of
the belief types of men and women in the sample of colorectal
cancer patients and the healthy controls. The purpose was to
examine to what extent the separate samples of men and women
are sufficiently similar so that they can be used in further com-
parisons with patients presenting other kinds of cancer diag-
noses. The comparisons show that men and women both in the
sample of colorectal cancer patients and the healthy controls
differ significantly only in the mean scores of beliefs about
norms. In both cases men score higher than women. The fact
that the gender difference appears in both samples in regard to
the same variable and has the same directionality reduces its
probable impact on further analyses with these variables.
Tables 3 and 4 present the results of the analyses that refer to
the hypotheses of the study. Table 3 presents the results of
comparing the male samples of colorectal cancer patients, pro-
state cancer patients and healthy controls. It shows that in re-
gard to all four types of beliefs the overall mean differences are
significant, and that the colorectal cancer patients differ from
both the prostate cancer patients and the healthy controls, but
the prostate cancer patients do not differ significantly from the
healthy controls. These results conform fully to those hypothe-
sized and expected.
Table 4 presents the results of comparing the female samples
of colorectal cancer patients, breast cancer patients and healthy
controls. It shows that in regard to all four types of beliefs the
overall mean differences are significant, and that the colorectal
cancer patients differ from both the breast cancer patients and
the healthy controls, but the breast cancer patients do not differ
significantly from the healthy controls. These results conform
fully to those hypothesized and expected.
Another analysis was performed for comparing the mean
scores of the prostate cancer patients with those of the breast
cancer patients (see column 2 in Table 3 and column 2 in Ta-
ble 4). The results showed that only in one belief type the mean
scores of these two groups differed significantly: in norm be-
liefs the mean score of the prostate cancer patients was higher
Table 2.
Comparing the means and Sd of the scores of men and women in the
four belief types in colorectal cancer patients and healthy controls.
Belief Type Colorectal Cancer Patients Healthy Controls
Men
(N = 101) Women
(N = 129) Men
(N = 55) Women
(N = 110)
Beliefs
about self 2.80 [.34] 2.78 [.33] 2.54 [.22] 2.50 [.28]
Beliefs
about norms 2.64 [.41] 2.47 [.36]a 2.36 [.27] 2.24 [.32] b
General
beliefs 2.73 [.36] 2.65 [.34] 2.42 [.23] 2.36 [.29]
Beliefs
about goals 2.79 [.40] 2.72 [.33] 2.52 [.27] 2.48 [.30]
Note. In columns 2-4 the number without brackets represents the mean score and
the number in brackets represents the Sd. aThe difference between the mean
scores of men and women is significant p < .001. bThe difference between the
mean scor es of men and women is sign ificant p < .05.
Table 3.
Comparing the means of the four belief types in the three samples of
men: colorectal cancer p atients, patients with p rostate cancer, and heal-
thy controls.
Belief Type Colorectal
Cancer
(N = 101)
Prostat e
Cancer
(N = 61)
Healthy
Controls
(N = 55)
F
DF = 2/214
Beliefs
about self 2.80 [.34] 2.58 [.21] 2. 54 [.22] 14.12***
Beliefs
about norms 2. 64 [ .41] 2.40 [ .30] 2.36 [. 2 7] 29.64***
General beliefs 2.73 [.36] 2.43 [.25] 2.42 [.23] 22.17***
Beliefs
about goals 2.79 [.40] 2.49 [.31] 2.52 [.27] 25.03***
Note. In columns 2-4 the number without brackets represents the mean score and
the number in brackets represents the Sd. ***p < .001. Comparisons of pairs of
means by the Scheffe post hoc method yielded significant results for the means of
colorectal cancer versus prostate cancer, and for colorectal cancer versus healthy
controls for each of the four belief types. In no case was the difference between
the means of prostate cancer and healthy controls significant.
Table 4.
Comparing the means of the four belief types in the three samples of
women: colorectal cancer patients, patients with breast cancer, and
healthy controls.
Belief Type Colorectal
Cancer
(N = 129)
Breast
Cancer
(N = 53)
Healthy
Controls
(N = 110)
F
DF = 2/289
Beliefs
about self 2.78 [.33] 2.52 [.21] 2.50 [. 2 8] 20.71***
Beliefs
about norms 2.47 [.36] 2.28 [.30] 2.24 [.32] 13.83***
General
beliefs 2.65 [.34] 2.41 [.31] 2.36 [ .29] 14.75***
Beliefs
about goals 2.72 [.33] 2.50 [.32] 2.48 [.30] 10.87***
Note. In columns 2-4 the number without brackets represents the mean score and
the number in brackets represents the Sd. ***p < .001. Comparisons of pairs of
means by the Scheffe post hoc method yielded significant results for the means of
colorectal cancer versus breast cancer, and for colorectal cancer versus healthy
controls for each of the four belief types. In no case was the difference between
the means of breast ca ncer and healthy controls significant.
S. KREITLER ET AL.
Open Access
85
than that of the breast cancer patients (p < .01). Notably the
mean score of norm beliefs was higher for mean than for
women also in the sample of colorectal cancer patients and in
the sample of healthy controls.
Discussion
The results of the presented study show that both hypotheses
were confirmed: the CO questionnaire of colorectal cancer dif-
ferentiated the male colorectal cancer patients from the healthy
controls and prostate cancer patients, but did not differentiate
the prostate cancer patients from the healthy controls. The same
was true of breast cancer patients: the CO questionnaire of co-
lorectal cancer differentiated the female colorectal cancer pa-
tients from the healthy controls and breast cancer patients, but
did not differentiate the breast cancer patients from the healthy
controls. Further, it did not differentiate between the prostate
cancer patients and the breast cancer patients except in regard
to beliefs about norms. Hence, it may be concluded that the CO
questionnaire of colorectal cancer is specifically targeted on
colorectal cancer and differentiates patients with this diagnosis
from healthy controls and from patients with other types of
cancer but does not differentiate the other types of cancer from
healthy controls.
The findings of the study provide a sturdy proof of the speci-
ficity of the CO of disease particularly in regard to colorectal
cancer. The proof is enhanced especially through the finding
that it refers to two types of cancer—prostate cancer and breast
cancer.
A notable result is the recurrent finding that men score higher
on norm beliefs in the samples of colorectal cancer, healthy
controls and in comparing prostate cancer patients with breast
cancer. In the sample of colorectal cancer patients the finding is
in accord with the evidence that in men colorectal cancer tends
to be a more severe disease than in women (Gao et al., 2008;
Kotake et al., 2003). However, this interpretation does not hold
for the finding in the other samples. Further research is needed
to clarify the reasons for this finding.
Conclusion
The major thrust of the results is that they contribute to
strengthening the claim that psychological risk factors defined
in terms of the CO theory are disease specific. The implications
of this result are important both in regard to the better under-
standing the disease itself and in regard to enhancing the role of
psychological risk factors in the domain of medicine.
Concerning the disease, it seems justified to conclude that
there exists a profile of psychological tendencies characteristic
of colorectal cancer. Its main characteristics are that it is a pro-
file and not one or another tendency, and that the tendencies
constitute together a matrix of dynamic interactions manifesting
how the individuals experience reality, how they handle them-
selves and how they interact with others. The major focus of
their cognitive orientation concerns striving to be perfect in
performing all one’s duties, commitments and direct as well as
indirect expectations of oneself. The further focus concerns the
relation to oneself as defined in terms of the polarity of self
assertion or self effacement. The relation to others is defined in
terms of the polarity of yearning for closeness and pleasing
others contrasted with the tendency to distancing oneself from
others. The polarities denote the constant potentiality for con-
flict if both contrasting tendencies are activated at the same
time. But if however, only one tendency is activated, the indi-
vidual may feel frustrated at not having expressed oneself fully
or completely. The potential conflicts form major sources of
tension.
In view of the findings of the present study it seems appro-
priate to suggest that the identified personality tendencies could
be considered as likely candidates for psychological risk factors
for colorectal cancer. The justification for this suggestion rests
on the evidence provided by the present study about the speci-
ficity of the identified psychological tendencies as well as on
previous findings, primarily those indicating that they do not
vary with various demographic variables, such as age, country
of origin or marital status; that they are independent of various
medical variables, mainly disease stage, being in treatment or in
remission, and mainly disease duration, which could potentially
change the individual’s behavior and emotional state; and they
are readily amenable to assessment by a reliable and valid tool.
The dictionary of cancer terms prepared by the National Cancer
Institute (NCI) defines risk factors as something that may in-
crease the chance of getting a disease, for example, risk factors
for cancer include age, a family history of certain cancers, use
of tobacco products, certain eating habits, obesity, lack of exer-
cise, exposure to radiation or other cancer-causing agents, and
certain genetic changes. The identified psychological tenden-
cies grounded in the CO theory seem to fit in very well within
this framework of risk factors.
REFERENCES
Amelang, M. (1997). Using personality variables to predict cancer and
heart disease. European Journal of Per s onality, 11, 319-342.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0984(199712)11:5<319::AID-
PER304>3.0.CO;2-D
Drechsler, I., Brunner, D., & Kreitler, S. (198 7). Cognitive antecedents
of coronary heart disease. Social Science and Medicine, 24, 581-588.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0277-9536(87)90062-1
Denollet, J. (1998). Personality and ri sk o f cancer in men with co ronary
heart disease. Psychological Medicine, 28, 991-995.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291797006442
Figer, A., Kreitler, S., Kreitler, M. M. et al. (2002). Personality disposi-
tions of colon cancer patients. Gastrointestinal Oncology, 4, 81-92.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1475956021000015095
Fox, B. (1998) Psychos ocial factors in cancer incidence and prognosis.
In J. Hollan d (Ed.), Psycho-oncology (pp. 110-124). New York: Ox-
ford University Press.
Gao, R. N., Neutel, C. I., & Wai, E. (2008). Gender differences in co-
lorectal cancer incidence, mortality, hospitalizations and surgical
procedures in Canada. Journal of Public Health, 30, 194-201.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/pubmed/fdn019
Goodkin, K., Antoni, M. H., Sevin, B., et al. (1993). A partially testable
predictive model of psychosocial factors in the etiology of cervical
cancer. I. Biological, psychological and social aspects. Psychoon-
cology, 2, 79-98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pon.2960020203
Kotake, K., Honjo, S., Sug ihara, K. et al. (2003). Changes in colorectal
cancer during a 20-year period: An extended report from the mul-
ti-institutional registry of large bowel cancer, Japan. Diseases of the
Colon & Rectum, 46, S32-S43.
Kreitler, H., & Kreitler, S. (1976). Cognitive orientation and behavior.
New York: Springer Publishing.
Kreitler, H., & Kreitler, S. (1982). The theory of cognitive orientation:
Widening the scope of behavior prediction. In B. Maher, & W. B.
Maher (Eds.), Progress in experimental personality research (Vol.
11, pp. 101-169). New York: Academic Press.
Kreitler, S. (2003). Psycho-oncology. In L. M. Cohen, D. E. McChar-
gue, & F. L. Collins Jr. (Eds.), The health psychology handbook (pp.
S. KREITLER ET AL.
Open Access
86
325-358). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.
Kreitler, S. (2004). The cognitive guidance of behavior. In J. T. J os t, M.
R. Banaji, & D. A. Prentice (Eds.), Perspectivism in social psychol-
ogy: The Yin and Yang of scientific progress (pp. 113-126). Wash-
ington DC: American Psychological Association.
Kreitler, S., & Kreitler, H. (1991a). Cognitive orientation and physical
disease or health. European Journal of Per s onality, 5, 109-129.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.2410050205
Kreitler, S., & Kreitler, H. (1991b). The psychological profile of the
health-oriented individual. European Journal of Personality, 5, 35-
60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.2410050104
Kreitler, S., & K reitler, H. (19 98). Cognitive orientation and disease in
cancer patients. Psychooncology, 1, 174.
Kreitler, S., Kreitler, H., & Chaitchik, S. (1993). Repressiven ess: Caus e
or result of cancer? Psychooncology, 2, 43-54.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pon.2960020107
Kreitler, S., Levavi, H., & Bornstein, G. (1996). Perso nality factors and
cervical premalignancy. Personality and Individual Differences, 21,
883-890. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00136-5
Kreitler, S., Kreitler, M . M., Len, A., Alkalay, Y., & Barak, F. (2008).
Psychological risk factors for colorectal cancer? Psycho-Oncologie,
2, 131-145. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11839-008-0094-9
Lilja, A. O., Smith, G., Malmstroem, P. et al. (1998). Psychological
profile related to malignant tumors of different histopathology. Psy-
chooncology, 7, 376-386.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1611(1998090)7:5<376::AID-
PON311>3.0.CO;2-Z
Nurymberg, K., Kreitler, S., & Weissler, K. (1996). The cognitive ori-
entation of compliance in short- and long-term type 2 diabetic pa-
tients. Patient Education and C ounseling, 29, 25-39.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0738-3991(96)00931-7
Sanderman, R., & Ranchor, A. V. (1997). The predictor status of per-
sonality variables: Etiolog ical significance and their role in the co ur-
se of disease. European Journal of Personality, 11, 359-382.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0984(199712)11:5<359::AID-
PER307>3.0.CO;2-0
Servaes, P., Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M, Vreugdenhil, G. et al. (1999). In-
hibition of emotional exp ressio n in b reast c ancer p ati en ts. Behavioral
Medicine, 25, 23-27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08964289909596735
Temoshok, L., Heller, B. W., Sagebiel, R. W. et al. (1985). The rela-
tionship of ps ychosocial factors to progn ostic indicators in cutan eous
malignant melanoma. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 29, 139-
153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-3999(85)90035-2