Open Journal of Political Science
2012. Vol.2, No.3, 45-58
Published Online October 2012 in SciRes (http://www.SciRP.org/journal/ojps) http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/ojps.2012.23006
Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 45
Common Origin, Common Power, or Common Life:
The Changing Landscape of Nationalisms
Agnes Katalin K oos
Political Science Department, Simon Fraser University, Vancouver, Canada
Email: agnes_koos@sfu.ca
Received June 21st, 2012; r evised July 11th, 2012; accepted Au g u st 1 1 th, 2012
Socio-territorial psychic constructs, such as national identities, are perhaps the most important psychic
phenomena for political science, with their strength so consequential for wars and inter-ethnic conflicts.
The construction of the EU has faced scholars and practitioners with two identity-related problems: (i)
whether the socio-territorial identities can be conceptualized as being multi-layered (nested, hyphenated,
with non-conflictual relationships among the components), and (ii) whether the higher levels of these
identity constructs can be confined to civic aspects (e.g. to a Habermasian constitutional patriotism), as
opposed to traditional nationalisms relying on assumptions of common origin, and shared culture. The
most entrenched classification of nationalisms relies on an obvious difference between the kinds of na-
tionalisms endorsed by the Irish and Germans, on one hand, and the French and white immigrant coun-
tries like the US, on the other hand. These versions are generally labeled “ethnocultural,” involving the
consciousness of a shared ancestry and history, and “civic”, relying on the idea of belonging to the same
state. My argument is that a schism within the “civic” approach to nationalism can theoretically be ex-
pected and empirically supported on the basis of the ISSP 2003, Eurobarometer 57.2 and 73.3 surveys.
These datasets confirm the existence of three principal components of nationalism, which can be labeled
“ethnocultural”, “great-power-civic” and “welfare-civic”. While the great-power-civic approach is con-
cerned with and takes pride in the country’s military strength, international influence, sovereignty, and
national character, the welfare-civic approach takes a more civilian stance and it is concerned with com-
mon rights, fair treatment of groups, social security, and welfare within the country. In addition, support
has been found for the assumption that people tend to construct their supra-national identity layer accord-
ing to the molds for their national identity.
Keywords: Socio-Territorial Identities; Nationalism; European Identity
Introduction
Socio-territorial psychic constructs, such as national identi-
ties, are perhaps the most important psychic phenomena for
political science, with their strength so consequential for wars
and inter-ethnic conflicts. Yet, political science joined in the
scholarly preoccupation with socio-territorial identities only
recently. Among the great political science paradigms, interna-
tional relations realism has taken nati onali sms for u ncha llenge d
givens, though of different intensity in different states. Interna-
tional relations liberalism, after a short but significant concern
with the self-determination of ethnic groups after the 1st World
War, only renewed interest in ethnic and minority arrangements
toward the end of the 20th century. In comparative politics,
Modernists relegated nation formation to a pre-mass politics
and pre-democracy era, thus concern with it has been dropped
from the study of modern political systems. Nationalism in
advanced settings became an important topic when the func-
tionalist dreams about the formation of a European identity
started to become true. In the previous decades, though, scho-
lars and politicians were confronted with the opposing propos-
als of Donald Horowitz and Arend Lijphart with regard to con-
stitutional engineering in divided societies1.
The major theoretical debates on socio-territorial identities
have been unfolding in other social sciences, particularly soci-
ology. By the 1990s the “foundational” debate of the field be-
tween primordialist and constructivist positions was largely
concluded. Political science tended to import constructivist
viewpoints, but these had a difficult time in making their way
into the core theoretical and methodological frames of the dis-
cipline focused on a Westphalian state system. We may recall
Kanchan Chandra’s (2001) amazement that the findings of the
modernist-constructivist camp “are being conspicuously and
comprehensively ignored in new research linking ethnic groups
to political and economic outcomes” (p. 7).
From a political science point of view, the time horizon
adopted by alternative explanations is very important. For pri-
mordialists (perennialists), socio-territorial identities are im-
mutable, exogenous to all other social phenomena, and they are
1Horowitz, in the true spirit of Modernism, looked for possibilities to reduce
the impact o f et hni c cleav ages on pol itical o u tco mes. Ass umin g t hat deepen -
ing cleavages threaten political stability, he championed systems that i)
reward moderation and penalize extremism; ii) encourage cross-ethnic
cooperation, such as channel ethnic tensions in two large parties along the
Left/Right dimension; and iii) divide state power along non-ethniclines.
Horowitz thought that cross-cutting cleavages mitigated ethnic conflict, but
many other theorists doubted that his formulas were conducive to inter-
ethnic accommodation. Lijphart, for instance, proposed “consociational”
mechanisms, t hat is , p ow er s har ing along eth ni c lin es. I n th is t heo r y, so lvin g
conflicts, rather than artificially silencing them, is the key to long-term
political stability and inter-ethnic peace. There is an ongoing debate about
whether consociationalism reinforces ethnic divides or not. It does not seem
to fuel furthe r animosity; but it may boost ethnic (group) consciousnesses.
A. K. KOOS
master identities in relation to other collective identities. For
constructivists2, identities are malleable, subjected to a number
of social influences, including political institutions, and people
may have non-hierarchically ranked multiple collective identi-
ties, among them multiple socio-territorial identities. The lite-
rature focused on socio-territorial identities regularly takes
sides in this debate, and slowly a dimension with many inter-
mediate values has come about, for instance, Anthony Smith’s
ethno-symbolism being placed at the middle of the scale. The
perennialist versus constructivist stance has a bearing on how
nation is defined, and on estimates of time necessary for na-
tion-formation. The basic schism in the conceptualization of
nation, traditionally mapped out as ethnocultural nationalism
versus civic nationalism, is inherently related to the paradig-
matic divide. Ethnocultural nationalisms tend to see themselves
as perennial entities, and assign a much longer time scale to
nation-formation than civic nationalisms do.
Accepting the idea that collective identities are subject to
change, we have to assume that they do so constantly. In the
case of national identities, it is their original creation that has
received the most attention. But the maintenance of national
identities does also suppose a coherent social discourse that
places a premium on being a patriot. In the words of Ernest
Renan, “nations are daily plebiscites. With changing interna-
tional and domestic institutions, 21st century nationalisms can-
not be identical with 19th century nationalisms. They are also
likely to have another status within the individual psyches than
in the previous centuries. Accurately taken, it is not only the
nationalisms, but the whole complex of socio-territorial identi-
ties that are changing. The socio-territorial identity complex
may be described as including, besides nationalism, the “lo-
cal-patriotism” of attitudes toward locality and region, includ-
ing minority group consciousnesses, either ethnocultural or of
an other type, as well as attitudes toward the above-national
fora such as the United Nations.
In general, I would argue that in today’s world, and mainly in
its more developed parts, i) the salience of socio-territorial
identities, in general, declines; 3ii) the need for a unique master
identity is less and less supported; and iii) the role of nationa-
lism within the socio-territorial identity complex retrogrades.
The emotional attachment to locality and region has always
been measured as being very close to patriotism values. Also,
with globalization and with regional integrations, people do not
believe anymore that all decisions pertinent to their lives should
or could be taken on the nation-state level. This relates to the
cognitive component of socio-territorial identities. The impor-
tance of a group for an individual hinges on the tasks that the
respective group is supposed to address. State abilities have
been eclipsed by globalization, no wonder people reformulate
their expectations toward it.
More narrowly, this paper aims at studying the types or ver-
sions of national identity, by gauging their historical dynamics,
as well. The empirical data for analyses comes from the Inter-
national Social Survey Program’s (ISSP) 2003 round on Na-
tional Identities and two Eurobarometer surveys, namely EB
57.2 (of 2002), and EB 73.3 (of 2010). The EU construction is
a great opportunity for studying changes of socio-territorial
identities, their causes and consequences. First, there are large
scale changes of institutions, the state attributions being dele-
gated onto lower and higher levels, that is, to sub-national and
supra-national fora. And second, there is a conscious concern
with stimulating the formation of a European identity, widely
regarded as a requisite of democratization within the super-
polity.
Hypotheses
National identities may be constructed in many ways, and
their meanings (both denotation and connotation) vary across
individuals. Yet, they may be deemed to display typical pat-
terns across countries or across some groups, such as minorities,
classes, and professional groups. The overwhelming majority of
the literature authorizes a distinction between “ethnocultural”
and “civic” versions of nationalism. While the first focuses on
common ancestry and common (language and literature related-)
culture, the second focuses on being citizens of the same state.
Often, the ethnocultural version is taken for being more primi-
tive and more dangerous, than civic nationalism. Yet, civic
nationalisms (most typically in white immigrant countries) can
be blamed for other things, such as, i) failure to generate levels
of social solidarity comparable with those in ethnically ho-
mogenous countries, in order to achieve welfare states; and ii)
expected and implemented assimilationism, which might have
been called a “melting pot” effect, but has always been carried
out under the domination of a certain culture, such as white
Anglo-Saxon Protestant in the US. Finally, there is no evidence
that any of the nationalism versions would be more peaceful;
high levels of militarism can occur in both4.
In his 2000 book, David Brown forwarded a comprehensive
scheme to classify nationalisms, which includes a third version,
“multicultural, as well (Figure 1). The scheme seems to be
drawn from the point of view of majority policy makers taking
into account the ethnic composition of their societies. In terms
of constitutional engineering, civic nationalism endorses a
Horowitzian, while multicultural nationalism, a Lijphartian pro-
gram.
Multicultural nationalism has been an answer to the social
fact of ethnic fragmentation, leading to institutional transforma-
tions such as “pillarization” in the Benelux states. But the
causal arrows run both ways: the political institutions affect
nationalisms, as well. Socio-territorial identities have to incor-
2We may conceive of the constructivist school in the field of collective
identities as of a middle-range sociological theory. It is supported by such
distinct comprehensive paradigms as functionalism/Modernism, Marxism,
feminism, postmodernism, and institutionalism. Weberianism seems to be
ambiguous w ith rega r d t o it .
3My emphasis is not on the strength of nationalist feelings (though I assume
that these decrease, as well), but on the relationship of nationalism with
other collective identities, such as social class or professional group. I be-
lieve that this relative importance of national belonging is decreasing with
time. This belief can survive counterexamples such as the outburst of na-
tionalisms in former Yugoslavia and parts of former Soviet Union in the
1990s. On the one hand, I tend to endorse Tilly’s explanation that crumbling
p
olitical authority triggers communal rivalry. And on the other hand, it is to
be noted that all successor nation-states chose to join “federations” later:
they became ei t h er p ar t s of t h e CI S, or members of t h e EU, o r can d id at es f o r
EU membership.
4Still in the constructivist tradition, we should admit that the endorsement o
f
kinds of nationalisms in different states is not accidental, but can be ren-
dered meaningful by national histories. I would suggest that it is related to
the presence of minorities within, or absence of co-nationals from a given
state. Ethno-cultural nationalism has been endorsed by peoples with large
proportion of co-nationals living abroad (Former West-Germany, Ireland,
Hungary, Greece), while the civic-
p
atriotic version has been promoted by
countries without significant percentage of co-ethnics living abroad as mi-
norities, but significant regional and ethnic variation inside the country
(France, It aly, and t h e immigrant countries).
Copyright © 2012 SciRes.
46
A. K. KOOS
Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 47
a:
Figure 1.
Brown’s (2000) class i f ic a tion scheme of nationalisms.
porate notions of territorial institutions, most notably a notion
of state. Since states have changed a great deal in the contem-
porary world, socio-territorial identities are bound to rely on
different meanings of state-ness. Of the above mentioned ver-
sions of nationalism, it is the civic-type that may be expected to
be the most susceptible to changes of state-ness. Basically,
civic nationalism can be assumed to co-vary with transforma-
tions of the state.
being constructed.
Democratic Welfare Nationalism
It has been a distinctive feature of international relations libe-
ralism, particularly of the democratic peace proposal, to point
out the impact of domestic regimes on the behavior of states in
the international arena. International relations constructivists
assisted liberals with the claim that institutions influence the
ways in which states (or their representatives in foreign policy
decision-making) perceive their “national interest, and define
their relations with other states. Yet, none of the theories
showed particular interest in the content and strength of nationa-
lisms, and none has traditionally elaborated on the idea that in
an era of mass democracy, people’s national identities may
become causally consequential for developments in interna-
tional relations5. The functionalist proposal for European inte-
gration in the original Mitrany tradition, and as articulated by
Deutsch, came very close to this thesis, but later neo-functiona-
list turns dropped the interest in mass opinion on behalf of the
interest in elite behavior. It was only in the 1990s that political
science renewed its explorations in socio-territorial identities,
ostensibly related to developments within the EU, as the most
advanced regional integration. In the post-Maastricht world,
little opposition remained to the idea that the future of integra-
tion hinges on popular support for the project, both for
enlargement and deepening of the cooperation among the “ever
closer” states. Further, this popular support has increasingly
come to be conceptualized in terms of European identity versus
national identity. Alternative explanations, such as utilitarian
ones, cannot circumvent the issue of whether people cling more
to payoffs to the nation, the individual, or maybe to some par-
ticular group, in their cost-benefit calculi? Opinion survey data
show that people tend to place as much premium on payoffs to
their nation as on benefits to themselves as individuals (Koos,
2007), while minorities such as Catalans in Spain and Scots in
But we know little about what changes in state structures and
in state roles affect socio-territorial identities, and how they
affect them. The EU construction shed light on some related
concerns, but has not led to a systematic inquiry in this sense.
The major problems raised by integration are: 1) whether
socio-territorial identities can be conceptualized as being multi-
layered (nested, hyphenated, with non-conflictual relationships
among the components); and 2) whether the higher levels of
these identity constructs can be confined to civic aspects (such
as to a Habermasian constitutional patriotism), as opposed to
ethnocultural nationalisms relying on assumptions of common
origin and shared culture, but also civic nationalisms premised
on a unitary state language and some shared political under-
standings.
From the other side, changes of state-ness in the world have
mainly been analyzed in two types of literature: effects of globa-
lization, on the one hand, and regional integration, on the other.
In Caporaso’s account of Westphalian, regulatory and post-
modern states (1996), the two seem to come together. An im-
portant insight of this study was that in both regulatory and
postmodern states, labor’s bargaining power decreases. Other
evidence also suggests that labor is at its best political efficacy
and most likely to a chieve full-fledged welfare arrangements in
smaller, homogenous nation-states, that is, in the traditional
Westphalian states.
This paper argues for two related claims: 1) once a welfare
state is achieved, it has an impact on how people construct their
nationalism; and 2) predominant senses of nationalism (and/or
of sub-national socio-territorial identities) in a state have an
impact on how supranational socio-territorial identities are
5Obviously within institutional settings that turn the principle into reality,
such as increasing reliance on r eferenda in f oreign policy decision making.
A. K. KOOS
Britain may be, in addition, grateful to the EU for fostering
decentralization policies in the Member States.
Renewed interest in socio-territorial identities does not mean,
however, either the comeback of a single dominant paradigm in
this field, or the existence of a generally accepted corpus of
accumulating knowledge. The fundamental cleavage between
primordialism and constructivism persists6, and inquiries into
the content, forms, and causes of nationalism follow several
paradigms. I would claim that there is a mainstream scholarship
endorsing the idea that nationalism comes in several versions,
and an important distinction can be made between its
ethno-cultural and civic forms, a creed promoted by Brubaker
(1992, 1996) and Greenfeld & Eastwood (2007), for instance.
Yet, we have to allow for the existence of some opposition to
this thesis. There have been arguments forwarded, for instance,
to de-emphasize the importance of this cleavage on grounds
that the two versions are each other’s strategic alternative in
determined conditions (Niklas, 1999). Greenfeld (1992) found a
cross-cutting cleavage, called individualism versus collectivism.
And this paper intends to argue for the existence of an increas-
ing difference between two types of civic nationalism.
I subscribe to the constructivist tradition, and in this tradition
we cannot exclude the political institutions from the determi-
nants of socio-territorial identities. The content of national
identity is rightly expected to be functional for the interests and
political goals of the collectivity for which it has been elabo-
rated, while the salience of national identity, to vary in function
of the social-political tensions faced by that collectivity. Cross-
country differences of ethnic fragmentation, regime, and insti-
tutions have led to obvious differences of the ways in which
people construct their national identities. The last great trans-
formation of the nation-state—previous to the triumph of glob-
alization and the postmodern governance of regional integra-
tions—has been the implementation of the democratic welfare
state in many developed countries. The consequences of this
institutional change can be expected to be in the direction of
“butter rather than guns. There are many arguments supporting
this expectation. i) Democracies do not fight each other, they
have been shown to be inherently more peaceful and/or more
circumspect with regard to war; and ii) The number and depth
of treaties tying together the developed countries creates an
international safety belt around them, which is regarded with
much less skepticism by the large masses than by international
relations realists. Actually, since the 1960s, Western Europe
has consistently refused to increase its military spending, and
after the end of the Cold War, both NATO and the OECD
group reduced it7.
Corresponding to these changes, and in an intricate two-way
relationship with the institutional evolution, nationalisms in
democratic welfare states can be expected to be (H1) less fueled
by military imagery and by notions of necessary confrontation
with other countries, and (H2) incorporate more sense of
socio-economic solidarity. It is to be noted that militarism is a
very potent cement to tie people together, as Raymond Aron’s
maxim that “wars are the midwives of nations” put it. In wel-
fare states, the militarist appeal is replaced with the possibility
of a participatory political culture, and belongingness to a
“from cradle to grave” welfare system. Other phenomena of
importance, such as the ascension of a by default international
environmentalism, and concern for the Third World, or at least
for their own former colonies, may nuance the picture of new
nationalisms.
Layers of Socio-Territorial Identities
Socio-territorial identities can be described as being verti-
cally multi-layered. Most of the literature allows for types of
complex and non-conflictual relationship between loyalty to
locality, region, and country. This has also been measured re-
peatedly, and sub-national loyalties in general have been found
to co-vary with national attachment. There was no serious
theoretical challenge formulated against these claims until the
emergence of the problem of supra- (or post, or inter, or trans-)
national indentities. The extension of the non-conflictual multi-
layered structure in order to incorporate continent-wide polities
has met severe resistance. Euroskepticism denies the possibility
of a potent European identity, and challenges the whole theory
allowing for multiple identities, as claimed, for instance, by
functionalists. The main argument of the skeptics, as formu-
lated, for instance, by Spiering (1999), is that people may have
only one “layer” of identity, which is really salient and domi-
nates all others. Actually, Spiering posits that people may have
only one core identity, while they may identify themselves,
very superficially, with many other groups. Spiering fails to
distinguish between personal identity and master identity. Ob-
viously, none denies the centrality of personal identities. Con-
structivist supporters of multiple and multi-layered identity
challenge the necessity of a master identity among collective
identities. Marc Glendening (2005), a leader of the Euroskeptic
Democracy Movement, argues for the necessity of a master
identity on grounds that we have to unambiguously locate po-
litical power at some level8. I do not think that psychology
supports that locating power is a prerequisite for forming
socio-territorial allegiances. Nevertheless, the modern world
has made it difficult to locate: there are multiple power struc-
tures in each society (such as economic, religious, and scientific,
besides the governmental authority) and the pure governmental
authority itself tends to be divided, horizontally in consocia-
tional arrangements, and vertically in federalism, decentraliza-
tion and subsidiarity. All opinion surveys show that people are
able to have and do have multiple collective identities. Yet,
there is a seed of truth in relating attachment and power. We
may expect people to emotionally endorse only a polity over
which they may have some influence9. But deeply felt lack of
8In Glending’s own words: “The other great political virtual reality claim the
PMAs [Post-Modern Authoritarianism] make is that in the globalised world
we can have “multiple identities”. We can simultaneously be citizens of our
regions , countries, the EU and the Wor ld . I t ’s no t a cas e of “either /o r ”, t hat’s
old-fashioned dualistic thinking, apparently. Political identity is dishonestly
being spun here as if it was directly analogous to one’s capacity to appreci-
ate the contrasting joys of both, say, thrash metal and trad jazz. As members
of governed societies, we have to choose where we believe ultimate, end o
f
the line, political authority should lie and which is the political community
we give our allegiance to. Music lovers don’t face such necessary choices.”
9In order to be equ al with oth ers in that polit y, no t margin alized, and not alien ated
This is an asp ect of the probl em of legitimac y, largel y debated wit hin and out side
the EU.
6Yet, the cleavage is often labeled with other terms, such as primordialism
versus instrumentalism, or perennialism versus Modernism. Though the
constructivist/Modernist/instrumentalist camp seems larger, it is more di-
vided, and there are well known primordialists opposing it, such as S. Van
Evera.
7On a global scale, the big ex ception is the US, whi ch, despite some red uc-
tion around 1990, kept increasing its military expenditures up to the latest
years. For instance, in 1994 the US devoted 4.3 percent of its GNP to de-
fense while the Non-US OECD average was only 1.8 percent and the
N
on-US NATO average was 2.4 percent.
Copyright © 2012 SciRes.
48
A. K. KOOS
Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 49
the ways in which people tend to construct their European iden-
tity, and
efficacy (or a feeling of alienation) can be shown to be associa-
ted with preference for the lowest levels of decision (local and
regional), not for the national level. (H4) The relationship between national identities and Euro-
pean identity hinges on the type of nationalism endorsed.
Even if we allow for the possibility of multiple collective
identities, and within them, for multiple socio-territorial identi-
ties, the concrete organization of component allegiances re-
mains open for discussion. Belief in the existence of (non-tri-
vial) multiple socio-territorial identity structures is regularly
associated with the creed that the component allegiances are not
in zero-sum terms. Evidence shows, for instance, that the
sub-national and national “layers” tend to co-vary across coun-
tries and age groups. In certain cases all three are weaker, in
other cases all three are stronger; they do not threaten each
other. Yet, beyond non-conflictuality, there is not much con-
sensus with regard to the relations among the elements of a
socio-territorial loyalty structure. Walzer (1990) prefers the
term “hyphenated, while others elaborate on “nested” socio-
territorial identities, in which the levels subsume each other
(Herb & Kaplan, 1999; Medrano & Gutierrez, 2001). Though
my basic insight of socio-territorial identity structures is very
close to a nested model, I prefer to call it “multilayered. In
some cases, the “layers” are not overlapping or including each
other, but rather cross-cutting. For instance, Germans in Alsace
may feel attached to their region, to France and to the EU, but
their attachment to a German culture protrudes from this loyalty
structure, fortunately not beyond the upper (EU) level.
The next section intends to bring empirical support for these
hypotheses. The two most comprehensive datasets to rely on
are a Eurobarometer of 2002 (#57.2) and the International So-
cial Survey Program’s 2003 round on National Identities. There
is no newer cross-national data allowing for a comprehensive
test of my claims. A Eurobarometer of 2010 (73.3), which has
inquired about national identities, may support previous find-
ings, but with a questionnaire much more reduced in compari-
son to the two main sources of data.
Data and Findings
(A) Eurobarometer 57.2 (2002) was carried out in 21 coun-
tries, but the battery on national identities was asked in 10
countries only: in West Germany, Greece, Italy, Spain, Great
Britain, East Germany, Austria, Czech Republic, Hungary, and
Poland. The questions of interest clustered under the numbers
Q25 - Q27. Question 25 asked about “how close10 do you feel
the following groups of people, the alternatives including local,
regional, national, and continent-wide groups (Illustration 1 ).
Findings on the question battery Q25 reinforce previous re-
sults obtained with similar questionnaires (Figure 2).
One more theoretical concern with the nested identity struc-
tures was raised by Risse (2005). What if the identity compo-
nents influence each other, mesh and blend into each other?
Risse calls this a “marble-cake” model of identity. I am sure
that this is the case. Concretely, in the case of the EU, a form-
ing European identity changes the meanings and emphases of
national and sub-national identities, though not necessarily the
strength of attachment to them. And the causal arrows run both
ways. The existing national and other socio-territorial identities
and perceived interests shape the ways in which people relate to
the EU. Part of Risse’s insight about a marble-cake model of
socio-territorial identity structure can be operationalized for the
EU with two hypotheses:
Attachment to locality, region and nationality co-vary closely.
Attachment to EU citizens and Europeans is significantly farther
Illustration 1.
Questions on socio-t erritorial attachments in EB 57.2.
1) The in ha bit a nt s of the c ity or village where you live/have lived most
of your life
2) The inhabitants of the region where you live (e.g. in UK: Scotland)
3) Fellow (NATIONALITY)
4) European Union citizens
5) Fellow Europeans (including European Union citizens and
people living in countries that are part of the European continent
but which may not make up part of the European Union)
6) People from Central and Eastern Europe
10
)
United States’citizens
(H3) Traditions of nationalism-constructs in countries shape
F eelin g clo se to ...
0.00
0.50
1.00
1.50
2.00
2.50
3.00
3.50
4.00
GREAT
BRITAIN
WEST
GERMA NY
CZECH
REPUBLIC
EAST
GERMA NY
AUSTRIAAverageITALYGREECESPAINPOLAND HUNGARY
CITY/ VILLAGE
REGION
NAT IONALIT Y
EU CITIZENS
EUROPEA NS
E.STN EUROPEANS
US CITIZENS
Feeling close to…
Figure 2.
Country means of variable battery Q25, “F ee li n g cl o se t o… ” .
10Very clo s e 1, Quite close 2, Not very close 3, Not at all close 4.
A. K. KOOS
from the attachment to this triad. Yet, these latter also seem to
show a co-varying pattern with the local and regional attach-
ments, that is, in some countries the socio-territorial loyalties
are stronger than in others in general. For instance, in Italy,
Spain and Hungary, attachment to EU citizens comes close to
what the British feel for their co-nationals.
Questions 26 and 27 asked about the reasons for feeling close to
certain groups, namely, to someone’s national group and to Euro-
peans (Illustration 2). The lists of alternatives given to respondents
paralleled each other to a considerable exten t, though not perfectly.
The question battery Q26 was subjected to a principal com-
ponent analysis. The results of a Varimax rotation of three fac-
tors cumulatively explaining 74.44 percent of variance are dis-
played in Table 1.
Illustration 2.
Questions on national and European identity in EB 57.2.
Q.26. Different things or feelings are crucial to people in their sense of
belonging to a nation. To what extent do you agree with the following
statements? 11 “I feel (Nationality) because I share with my fellow (Nationa-
lity)...”
Q.27. Different things or feelings are crucial to people in their sense of
belonging to Europe. To what extent do you agree with the following state-
ments?
Q.26 Q.27
1. I do not feel
(Nationality) 1. I do not feel European
2. a common culture,
customs & traditions 2. a common c ivilisation
3. a common language 3. membership in a European society
with many languages and cultures
4. comm on ancestry 4. comm on ancestry
5. a common history and
a common destiny 5. a common history and a
common destiny
6. a common political and
legal syste m
6. the EU institutions and an
emerging common political
and legal system
7. comm on r igh ts and dutie s 7. comm on r igh ts and dutie s
8. a comm on s ystem of
social security/welfare 8. a common system of social
protection within the EU
9. a nationa l economy 9. the right to free movement a nd
residence in any part of th e EU
14a/14b. a common EU currency
10. a national army 10. an emerging EU defe n ce system
11. comm on borders 11. a common European
homeland
12. a feelin g of nationa l pride 12. a feeling of pride for being
European
13. national in dependence
and sovereign t y 13. sovere ignty of the EU
14. our national character
15. our national symbols (the
flag, the national anthem, etc.) 15. a set of EU symbols
(flag, anthem, etc.)
Although all variables have loadings on each factor, the three
principal components are clearly and meaningfully outlined. On
the basis of the loadings, it is easy to label them as “Ethnocul-
tural” (with above-average loadings of the variables of common
culture, language, ancestry and history); “Welfare civic” (with
above-average loadings of the variables of political system,
common rights, welfare system and economy), and “Great
power civic” (with above-average loadings of the variables of
army, borders, national pride, sovereignty, national character
and national symbols). This last label possibly needs some ex-
planation. Emphasis on the things belonging to this group re-
flects a concern with a state at continuous conflict with other
states, in the spirit of international relations realism. And this
discipline is focused on giving advice to great powers, not to
small and weak countries12.
Interestingly, variables on national character do not load
heavily on the “Ethnocultural” factor. Although primordialists
claim that common culture, language, ancestry and history
shape people similarly, theories of national character have been
developed within a political competition-centered context,
rather than within the descriptive context characteristic of the
original Herderian ethno-cultural primordialism13. If the con-
cept of national character has become politicized during the
centuries, national symbols have constantly been adjusted to the
political reality of borders and institutions in place. No wonder
they load much heavier on the great-power-civic than on the
ethno-cultural factor.
Next, a similar principal component analysis, with a Varimax
rotation, has been performed on the answers given to question
#27, that is, to the question about experiencing a feeling of
belonging to Europe. In this case, the first three factors explain
69.58 percent of variance. Though the principal components are
less sharply outlined than for the national loyalty, the results
suggest that the schisms and differences in constructing na-
tional identities continue in the realm of European identity, as
well. Otherwise, as shown in Table 2, European identity seems
to obey the rules of construction of other socio-territorial iden-
tities.
The next interesting question is whether there is any relationship
between the ways in which people construct their national iden-
tities, on one hand, and their European identity, on the other.
This may be studied with a correlation matrix of the two ques-
tion batteries, in which we arrange the variables according to
the three groups received with principal component analysis. In
such a matrix, higher values along the main diagonal mean
higher association between ethno-cultural national identity and
ethno-cultural European identity, or welfare-civic national
identity and welfare-civic European identity, as compared to
the associations within variables belonging to different princi-
pal components. It can be shown, that indeed, the average value
of the correlation coefficients along the main diagonal is 0.26
as ompared to the average of 0.19 of the off-diagonal correlation c
12Small and weak countries can hardly benefit of the bullying tactics rou-
tinely recommended by international relations realism. For economic as well
as for security reasons, these countries are better off near cooperative alli-
ance syst ems, includi ng regional integrations.
13Writing on the national character in 18th century France, historian P. Kra
(2002) notices the transition of the concept from descriptive to politically
activist: “At the beginning of the century national character was observed as
a histor ical fact; towards t he end it was regar ded as an active pol itical force
that must be fostered as the basis for reform. Thus national character moved
from the realm of speculation to that of theory with immediate practical
applications.”
11Strongly agree 1, Tend to agree 2, Tend to disagree 3, Strongly disagree 4.
Copyright © 2012 SciRes.
50
A. K. KOOS
Table 1.
Rotated Component Matrix of National Identity (N.ID).
Principal componen t
Component variable Great power civic 28.92% Welfare civi c 23.35% Ethnocultural 22.18%
N.ID: comm on culture 0.261 0.220 0.774
N.ID: common language 0.212 0.237 0.786
N.ID: comm on ancestry 0.301 0.234 0.789
N.ID: common history 0.313 0.301 0.714
N.ID: political system 0.260 0.737 0.372
N.ID: common rights 0.232 0.784 0.326
N.ID: welfare system 0.256 0.839 0.212
N.ID: national economy 0.381 0.759 0.202
N.ID: national army 0.688 0.470 0.115
N.ID: common borders 0.660 0.384 0.233
N.ID: national pride 0.821 0.212 0.291
N.ID: national sove reignty 0.763 0.304 0.265
N.ID: national character 0.781 0.226 0.331
N.ID: national symbols 0.800 0.155 0.303
Table 2.
Rotated Component Matrix of European Identity (EU.ID).
Principal componen t
Component variable Welfare civi c 24.13% Great power civic 23.48% Ethnocultural 21.97%
EU.ID: common civilisation 0.317 0.186 0.738
EU.ID: common ancestry 0.149 0.301 0.823
EU.ID: common history 0.209 0.293 0.784
EU.ID: society membership 0.478 0.196 0.616
EU.ID: political system 0.654 0.330 0.383
EU.ID: common rights 0.736 0.270 0.340
EU.ID: welfare system 0.696 0.292 0.348
EU.ID: free movement 0.765 0.281 0.160
EU.ID: common currency 0.504 0.598 0.046
EU.ID: common defence 0.568 0.515 0.208
EU.ID: common home land 0.366 0.618 0.334
EU.ID: pride 0.242 0.753 0.372
EU.ID: EU sovereignty 0.368 0.716 0.280
EU.ID: symbols 0.204 0.780 0.278
coefficients, which supports the claim that people socialized to
certain ways to construct their national identities tend to con-
struct their European identity along the same criteria of sali-
ence.
(B) ISSP 2003 on National Identities included 33 countries,
offering a larger base for generalizations than EB 57.2, but the
questions asked are less suitable for reconstructing the under-
lying dimensions of nationalist attitudes. Originally I intended
to rely on the battery concerning reasons for national pride, but
I had to realize that this battery lacks references to an ethno-
cultural construction of national allegiance. Thus, I merged the
battery on pride with a battery asking about the importance of
Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 51
A. K. KOOS
certain features for being “a good national14. The questions are
presented in Illustration 3.
The right column in Illustration 3 refers to the way in which
the variables have been assessed when interpreting the principal
components. While some of the answers are clearly pertinent to
the dimensions sought for (that is, ethnocultural, welfare, and
great-power civic) others may be associated with more of them.
On one hand, they are not very relevant for my tripartite classi-
fication, and on the other, they express commonsensical
knowledge (included either in expectations, or in reasons of
pride). For instance, “to be able to speak [country language]”
may sound like an ethnocultural requirement, but it also ex-
presses real, pragmatic concerns with communication. Respon-
dents have not been given the opportunity to distinguish be-
tween “speaking country language” and “being able to commu-
nicate with country nationals. On the pride battery, the items
“Its scientific and technological achievements, “Its achieve-
ments in sports, and “Its achievements in the arts and litera-
ture” have become pragmatic in virtue of their wording, that is,
because of their containing t h e word “achievem ents15.
In a first exploratory step I asked for comparing the country
means on these eighteen variables, and for clustering the coun-
tries on the basis of their means. All country means differences
have been found significant, and the clustering returned four
meaningful groups that reproduce the geographical zones in-
volved in this opinion survey. The following four groups have
emerged:
1) White immigrant/Anglo-Saxon, including the United
States, Australia, Canada, New Zealand, South Africa, Great
Britain, Ireland, with three odd ones: Austria, Spain, and Japan.
2) Western European & Nordic, including France, Ger-
many-East, Germany-West, Denmark, Finland, Sweden, Nor-
way, Switzerland, with two odd ones: Taiwan, and Israel Arabs.
3) Eastern European, including Slovenia, Czech Republic,
Slovak Republic, Hungary, Poland, Russia, with two odd ones:
Portugal, and South Korea.
4) Latin-American, including Chile, Uruguay, Venezuela,
with two odd ones: Philippines, and Israel Jews16.
Illustration 3.
Questions about content of nationalit y & reasons for pride in ISSP 2003.
Pertinent dimension(s)
Q.3: Some people say that the following things are i mportant for b eing truly [ nationality]. Others say they are no t i mportant.
How important do you think each of the following is...a
To have [c ou ntry nationality] ancestry ethnocultural
To have been born in [country] ethnocultural
To be a [religio n ] [of the dominant religion] ethnocultural
To have lived in [c. try] for most of one’s life ethnocultural/pragmatic
To be able t o s peak [countr y language] pragmatic/ethnocultural
To have [c o u n try nationality] citizenship civic/pragmatic
To respect... political institutions and laws civic/pragmatic
To feel [country nationality] pragmatic
Q.5: How proud are you of [country] in each of the following?b
[Country’s] a rmed forces great-power-civic
Its history great-power-civic
Its politica l i nf luence in the world great-power-civic/pragmatic
[Country’s] economic achievements pragmatic
The way democracy works welfare-civic/pragmatic
Its fair & equal treatment of all groups... welfare-civic
Its social security system welfare-civic
Its scientific and technolo gy achievements pragmatic
Its achievements in sports pragmatic
Its achievements in the arts and literature pragmatic
Note: aT he answer categories were: 1. Very im porta nt; 2. Fairly impor tant; 3. Not very important; 4. Not important at all; 8. DK ; 9. NA, refused.
bThe answer categories were: 1. Very proud; 2. Somewhat pr oud; 3. Not very proud; 4. Not proud at all; 8. DK; 9. NA, refused.
14This battery on “Importance”, on the other hand, lacks questions about military, glorious past and so on. I definitely needed both batteries in order to cover the
whole range of theoretically possible identity constructs. Yet, the juxtaposition of questions of two different types risks obtaining the answers belonging to the
same battery clustering together against the other battery, an artifice induced by the number of answer possibilities, wording, and so on. Some “cluster-
ing-together” effect seems to have emerged, indeed, but finally the meaning of the answers came through despite these technical hurdles .
15If it is an achievement, it is a reason for pride by default, independent of the domain. In the beginning, I hoped that the “achievements in the arts and litera-
ture” item can be used for measuring ethnocultural loyalty, but it turned out that that it shows high correlations only with the other “achievement” measures,
and not with the ethnocultural items (such as importance of ancestry) on the “Importance” battery.
16Bulgaria an d Latvia could not be i ncluded because of the lack of the cruci al question on ances try. In the 6-cluster resolution Taiwan and Israel Arabs b reak
free from the West-European/Nordic group, and so do Israel Jews from the Latin American group.
Copyright © 2012 SciRes.
52
A. K. KOOS
As my main concern here lies with the difference between
the ways in which populations of the advanced countries con-
struct their national identity, the principal component analysis
below presented is confined to seventeen countries with their
GDP per capita in purchase power parity above $25,000 in
2005. This means the first two groups without South Africa and
Israel, roughly half of the overall sample. In order to assure that
the seventeen peoples contribute to the factors in equal rates, I
used, in addition to the design weight provided by the authors
of the dataset, a sample correction weight, which rounded up all
national samples to 10,000.
In all factor resolutions conducted, a tension between focus
on military and focus on social security system could be ob-
served. Yet, because of their variegated associations with the
neutral (pragmatic) variables, the rank order of the factors be-
came easily reversed in function of the rotation technique (such
as Varimax or Quartimax) used. I sharpened and stabilized the
factors by cutting four variables from among the eighteen initial
ones. I renounced the “Important to feel national” item, and the
three “Pride-in-achievement” questions. On the basis of fourteen
variables, an SPSS Equamax rotation reported five principal
components cumulatively explaining 6 5.05% of variance. For t he
sake of visibility, Figure 3 summarizes the weight of the factors
and displays the factor loadings above 0.4 on each. Variables
belonging to the ethnocultural imagery are colored with shades of
blue, variables pertaining to a welfare-civic vision are shades of
red, and the great-power outlook-related variables are brown.
Undoubtedly, the opposition and tension between wel-
fare-civic and great-power civic outlooks exists in this sample
of seventeen developed countries. So as to connect these results
to the previous findings on the European Union, we may check
on the distribution of factor scores across countries. The coun-
try means of the factor-scores calculated by SPSS with the
Bartlett method are convergent with the initial clustering of the
countries into White immigrant & Anglo-Saxon and West-
European & Nordic (Table 3). Here it is mainly the “great
power civic” variable that shows a polarization in this sense.
No surprise that the US and Great Britain are the highest, and
the Germans and Nordics the lowest on it. The “welfare civic”
variable is topped by Canada, an immigrant country, but among
the countries with an above-average score on it we may notice
Switzerland, Denmark, Finland and Norway. Sweden, which
we could expect to score high on this variable, satisfies all other
related expectations (very low on great power civic, on ethnic,
and on religious factors), but here it scores mediocre. We may
wonder whether this is related to Sweden’s very high pragmati-
cism or not. It is beyond the reach of this paper to speculate
about the further evolution of nationalism, after its welfare-
civic type. A pure pragmatic approach to socio-territorial com-
munities, such as “my fellow men who respect the law and with
whom I can communicate, is one of the possibilities. It falls in
the direction of a Habermasian constitutional patriotism, but
leaves open the question of the source and content of laws to be
respected.
(C) The standard Eurobarometer of spring 2010, labeled EB
73.3, asked questions about national identity (QB1), European
identity (QB2), and also about whether the respondent felt as-
signed by others to any particular group based on his/her skin
color, ethnicity, religion, accent, lifestyle and so on (QB15). In
principle, these question batteries could have offered the possi-
bility to conduct an extensive factor analysis based on all three
batteries, but practically, the questions did not cover the whole
gamut of possibilities, such as the “great power” imagery has
completely been neglected, and the batteries did not well com-
plement each other to make comparisons possible. Illustration
4 presents these questions and the frequency with which the
respondents chose a certain answer, while Table 4 summarizes
the results of the principal component analysis.
All questions were formulated as yes/no dichotomies, thus
the value of the group mean of each equals the proportion of
respondents choosing that particular characteristic. This made
possible a fast review of the choices specific to certain coun-
tries, and the results are close to what could be expected. Table
5 provides a few examples in this sense. Social protection is
0.805
0.831
0.508
0.747
0.575
0.805
0.601
0.599
0.498
0. 447
0. 850
0. 695
0.594
0.452
0.505
0.655
0.443
0.754
0.788
0.415
0%
10%
20%
30%
40%
50%
60%
70%
80%
90%
1
00%
Ethnic -
18.48% Welfare civ ic -
13.06% Great power
civic - 11.6 8%R eligious -
11.07% Pragm a tic -
10.77%
Factor loadings on the first five principal components
P_milit
P_histry
P_p_infl
P_econ
P_demcry
I_citizn
I_rpi nst
P_fair
P_socss
I_speak
I_livein
I_relig
I_bornin
I_ancstry
Factor load ings on the firs t five princ ipa l compo ne nts
Figure 3.
Factor loadings above 0.4 on the f irst five principal components Data sourc e: ISSP 2003.
Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 53
A. K. KOOS
Table 3.
Country means of the fact o r scores calculat e d fo r the principal com p o n en t re s o l u ti o n i n Figure 3, based on ISSP 2003.
Ethnic Welfare civic Great power civic Religious Pragmatic
Austria (AT) –0.44 CA –0.41 US –0.75 US –0.76 SE –0.67
Ireland (IE) –0.42 AT –0.30 GB –0.64 IE –0.59 US –0.49
Japan (JP) –0.36 CH –0.29 IE –0.48 AU –0.26 NO –0.47
Spain (ES) –0.28 DK –0.28 AU –0.31 NZ –0.18 FR –0.46
New Zealand (NZ) –0.15 FI –0.14 CA –0.20 DE –0.06 DK –0.41
United States (U S) –0.10 NO –0.13 FR –0.18 GB –0.05 CA –0.35
Taiwan (TW) –0.04 ES –0.13 FI –0.14 ES –0.05 NZ –0.15
Denmark (DK) 0.02 AU 0.00 NZ –0.07 DK –0.05 AU –0.11
Canada (CA) 0.07 IE 0.04 ES 0.05 CH –0.04 AT –0.01
Norway (NO) 0.08 SE 0.11 JP 0.10 AT –0.02 DE 0.01
Germany (DE) 0.09 US 0.12 AT 0.24 CA 0.13 CH 0.04
Finland (FI) 0.13 DE 0.13 CH 0.27 NO 0.22 GB 0.04
Great Britain (GB) 0.13 NZ 0.15 TW 0.35 JP 0.32 FI 0.15
France (FR) 0.22 FR 0.16 DK 0.36 FI 0.32 TW 0.36
Switzerland (CH) 0.28 JP 0.16 NO 0.39 SE 0.47 ES 0.48
Australia (AU) 0.31 GB 0.25 SE 0.46 TW 0.53 JP 0.70
Sweden (SE) 0.58 TW 0.65 DE 0.94 FR 0. 55 IE 1.21
Illustration 4.
Socio-territorial identity questions in Eurobarometer 73.3.
National identity (QB1) Mentioned
% European identity
(QB2) Mentioned
%
Minority identity (QB15)
perceived assignment to
specific group
Mentioned
%
To have at least one
(Nationality) parents 17.9 Your skin color or ethn ic
origin 22.0
Your physic al condition or
appearance 14.3
Your name 14.8
To be a Chris ti an 8.8 Common religio u s
heritage 5.4 Your religion 17.1
To share (Nati o nality) cultural
traditions 32.7 Common culture 22.3 Your culture, values, lifestyle 29.8
Your clothe s , the way you
are dressed 9.1
To be born in (Our Country) 48.9
Geography 22.4
To have been brought up in
(Country) 28.0 Common history 17.3 Your social background 1 4.2
To master (CNTRY/OFF) language 34.0 Your language or accent 34.1
To feel (Nati o nality) 34.4
The single currency, the
Euro 36.4
To exercise citizens’ rights, e.g. vot-
ing 33.0 Democratic values 31.8
Being active in any association or
organization 3.5
A high level of social
protection 13.0
Symbols: flag, hymn and
motto 10.8
The area where you live 19.1
Your occupation 7.1
Your age 6.9
Note: Sourc e: EB 73.3, frequenc ies calcula ted by applying w22. The sample included 27 countries w/26, 602 respondents; multiple answers were allowed on all 3 items.
Copyright © 2012 SciRes.
54
A. K. KOOS
Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 55
Table 4.
Principal component analysis of the Eurobarometer 73.3’s questions about national and European identity.
Component
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
QB1 be Christian –0.004 0.000 0.102 0.754 –0.014 0.072 –0.163 –0.057 –0.003
QB1 place of birth –0.114 –0.173 –0.218 –0.015 0.417 0.050 –0.385 –0.346 0.008
QB1 feel nationality 0.011 0.062 0.076 –0.121
–0.881 0.069 –0.106 –0.132 0.050
QB1 cultural traditions 0.003 –0.037 0.426 0.051 –0.074 –0.467 0.126 0.027 –0.221
QB1 parentage 0.003 0.339 0.211 –0.201
0.449 0.325 –0.236 –0.228 0.098
QB1 country/official language 0.042 0.016 –0.029 –0.039 0.045 0.070 0.883 –0.107 0.024
QB1 citizens’ rights 0.111 –0.143 0.441 –0.178 0.115 –0.011 0.107 0.482 0.125
QB1 been brought up 0.006 –0.015 –0.792 –0.101 0.079 –0.071 0.059 0.058 –0.032
QB1 participation 0.006 0.055 –0.099 0.039 0.015 0.033 –0.125
0.825 –0.019
QB2 common history –0.251 0.627 0.031 0.032 –0.022 –0.069 0.074 0.044 0.042
QB2 geography –0.405 0.096 0.029 –0.135 –0.095 0.427 0.029 0.028 –0.628
QB2 democratic val u e s 0.604 –0.054 0.244 –0.136 0.044 –0.096 0.223 0.013 –0.090
QB2 social protection 0.747 0.020 –0.126 0.030 –0.081 0.173 –0.072 0.058 –0.019
QB2 common culture –0.121 0.252 –0.104 –0.093 0.040 –0.743 –0.134 –0.046 0.012
QB2 religious heritage –0.043 0.107 –0. 02 5 0.691 0.074 –0.040 0.112 0.040 0.023
QB2 common currency –0.236 –0.775 0.084 –0.121 0.043 0.112 0.024 0.008 0.088
QB2 symbols –0.211 0.011 0.027 –0.027 –0.068 0.177 0.034 0.034 0.783
Note: Source: EB 73.3. Results obtained with V arimax rotation, while weight w22 has been applied.
mainly valued in the Nordic and Post-communist countries, but
the Nordics add a strong desire for democracy to this. Religion
is more important in the Catholic and Orthodox world, than in
the Protestant areas.
Since the frequencies by countries are congruent with our as-
sumptions related to the construction of national and Euro-
pean identity in some countries, we had reasons to expect a
principal component analysis also confirming the related hy-
potheses. Indeed, Table 4 shows some support for them. Yet,
this time it took nine principal components to explain 63.3% of
variance, each component being responsible for 6% - 7% of it,
which means a quite dispersed distribution. Since there were
not questions probing the “great-power civic” attitude, here we
only may observe the tension between the “welfare-civic”
choices (components 1 and 8) and religious/cultural (compo-
nents 4 and 6). The other five principal components can be
described as centered on pragmatic things, such as common
currency and mastering the country’s official language.
Conclusion and Questions for Further
Research
The principal component analyses confirmed that nationa-
lism constructs in the EU and in the advanced countries in ge-
neral, have more significant factors than the generally expected
two, ethnocultural and civic. They reveal the existence of a kind
of nationalism based on the nation conceived of as a self-go-
verning community. This factor has emerged as one of the three
most important and meaningful principal components in analy-
ses. It is civic, as it does not involve the myth of common ori-
gin and ancestry, but the concept of state on which it relies
diverges from the international relations realist conception of
state.
The findings have also revealed a specific case of “institu-
tional inertia” or “path-dependence. Traditions of national-
ism-constructs in countries shape the ways in which people
tend to construct their European identity. The supra-national
layers are being cast in the old molds for national-and possibly
sub-national, an issue not tested here-identity, though these
latter do also change in time. Overall, the findings in this regard
do justice to Risse’s “marble cake” model. In addition, as people
show more attachment to EU citizens, than to Europeans, it is
possible to claim that a civic versus ethnocultural construction
of European identity is already preponderant in the EU. Unfor-
tunately, the data fails to provide conclusive evidence whether
the we-feeling toward EU citizens tends to be of the wel-
fare-civic or of the great-power-civic type. I think that the de-
gree of compatibility of ethnocultural nationalisms with a Eu-
ropean identity involves considerations about the role of the EU
in solving ethnic tensions. For instance, Irish ethnocultural na-
tionalism is not anti-European, as the EU has had a role in
mitigating the conflict in Norther Ireland. n
A. K. KOOS
Table 5.
Proportion of respondents choosing certain elements of European identity, by country.
Qb2
Social protection Qb2
Democratic values Qb2
Religious heritage
Mean Mean Mean
Lituania 0.29 Sweden 0.71 Poland 0.11
Sweden 0.26 Denmark 0.65 Italy 0.10
Latvia 0.25 Cyprus 0.51 Greece 0.09
Denmark 0.24 Germany W0. 0.47 Romania 0.09
Austria 0.23 Netherlands 0.46 Malta 0.08
Estonia 0.23 Luxembourg 0.44 Austria 0.06
Germany Easta 0.21 Austria 0.40 N0. Ireland 0.06
Germany West 0.20 Belgium 0.40 Cyprus 0.06
Cyprus 0.19 Germany E0. 0.38 Hungary 0.05
Finland 0.18 Finland 0.36 Slovakia 0.05
Luxembourg 0.17 France 0.35 Gr0. Britain 0.05
Netherlands 0.17 Lituania 0.34 Lithuania 0.04
Malta 0.16 Italy 0.32 Germany W0. 0.04
Belgium 0.16 Malta 0.32 Finland 0.04
Bulgaria 0.15 Czech Rep0. 0.31 Slovenia 0.04
Czech Rep0. 0.14 Hungary 0.31 Bulgaria 0.04
Hungary 0.14 Estonia 0.29 Germany E0. 0.03
Romania 0.14 N0. Ireland 0.29 Denmark 0.03
Slovakia 0.13 Bulgaria 0.28 Czech Rep0. 0.03
Italy 0.12 Slovakia 0.27 France 0.03
N0. Ireland 0.12 N0. Ireland 0.26 Netherlan ds 0.03
Greece 0.11 Gr0. Britain 0.25 Ireland 0.03
Ireland 0.10 Slovenia 0.25 Belgium 0.03
Slovenia 0.10 Latvia 0.23 Estonia 0.03
Gr0. Britain 0.10 Romania 0.23 Luxembour g 0.02
France 0.09 Greece 0.19 Spain 0.02
Spain 0.07 Portugal 0.18 Latvia 0.02
Poland 0.06 Poland 0.18 Portugal 0.02
Portugal 0.06 Spain 0.16 Sweden 0.02
EU average 0.16 EU average 0.33 EU average 0.05
Note: aThe Eurobarometer surveys still distinguish between East and West German samples, which may occasionally be very useful for tracing the heritage of a communist
ast. In this table, for instance, a “less-interest-in-democratic-values” effect comes through convincingly. p
Copyright © 2012 SciRes.
56
A. K. KOOS
The practical consequences of these findings for regional,
particularly European, integration are that a historical shift from
great-power civic nationalisms to welfare-civic nationalisms
works for the smooth inclusion of a supra-national layer into the
socio-territorial allegiance structure. The constitutional patriot-
ism-type European identity (as proposed by Habermas (2003))
may be the most successful in countries with a tradition of wel-
fare-civic nationalism. Countries with ethnocultural nationalism
traditions may experience more difficulty in integrating a consti-
tutional-patriotism-type European identity, but they won’t turn
against the EU until it is a solution to their ethnic problems.
Unfortunately, there is a serious tension arising from the fact
that citizens of the countries with the best welfare arrangements
are the most opposed to the neoliberal economic policies push-
ed forward by Brussels. Fear of downsizing the welfare system
has been at the heart of Nordic Euroskepticism. It seems that
the EU is seriously incapacitated in benefiting of the “cultural
match” between some countries welfare-civic nationalist out-
look and the shared values promoted in the self-definition of the
superpolity, and enshrined in the Charter of Fundamental
Rights.
Yet, the presence or absence of similar “cultural matches”
should not be omitted from the research agenda on which
European policies are based. Since we may claim that existing
types of nationalisms constrain the way in which European
identity is being constructed, logically we should admit that
types of nationalisms constrain the images of European integra-
tion, that is, the bl u ep ri n ts for integration endorsed by people.
As for further research in this domain, the empirical work is
constrained by the availability of survey data. Eurobarometer
57.2 is a unique dataset in the sense that it has been the only
one asking about the structure of a European allegiance thus far.
Unfortunately, it is confined to a small selection of European
countries, and its results cannot be safely generalized, not even
for the whole of the EU, let alone for beyond the continent.
ISSP 2003 provides more information about the emergence of a
welfare-civic nationalism in the global arena. Yet, despite the
much larger sample, ISSP does not contain really poor and
backward countries, or Muslim countries, for comprehensive
comparisons17. In addition, the survey questions are less suit-
able for reconstructing the underlying dimensions.
On the positive side, ISSP has the potential that the 2003
findings be compared with the earlier ISSP of 1995 on National
Identities, and with new results expected in 2013, when ISSP
intends to carry out a third wave of national identity research.
These analyses could give us clues about the temporal direction
and speed of change of nationalisms.
Socio-territorial loyalties are strong forces with a sad his-
torical record of having authorized deadly violence, as well as
other inhuman means applied to “outgroups. We cannot
over-emphasize the importance of obtaining unbiased, reliable
knowledge about the nature and dynamics of these allegiances.
Less dramatically, within the EU, more knowledge about na-
tionalisms and their relationship with a supranational identity,
can rightly be expected to foster sounder policies of integration.
REFERENCES
Brown, D. (2000). Contemporary nationalism: Civic, ethnocultural,
and multicultural politics. London: Rou tle dg e.
Brubaker, R. (1992). Citizenship and nationhood in France and Ger-
many. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
doi:10.1017/CBO9780511558764
Brubaker, R. (1996). Nationalism reframed: Nationhood and the na-
tional question in the New Europe. Cambridge, MA: Cambridg e Un i-
versity Press.
Caporaso, J. (1996). The European Union and forms of state: West-
phalian, regulatory or post-modern? Journal of Common Market Stu-
dies, 34, 29-52. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5965.1996.tb00559.x
Chandra, K. (2001). Introduction: Constructivist findings and their
non-incorporation. Symposium: Cumulative Findings in the Study of
Ethnic Politics, 12, 7-11.
Glendening, M. (2005). Post-modernism & the silent revolution. Euro-
pean Journal, 2005.
http://www.democracymovement.org.uk/postmod
Greenfeld, L. (1992). Nationalism: Five roads to modernity. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Greenfeld, L., & Eastwood, J. (2007). National identity. In C. Boix, &
S. Stokes (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of comparative politics, New
York: Oxford.
Habermas, J., & Derrida, J. (2003). February 15, or what binds Euro-
peans together: A plea for a common foreign policy, beginning in the
core of Europe, Constellations: An International Journal of Critical
& Democratic Theory, 10, 291-298. doi:10.1111/1467-8675.00333
Herb G. H., & Kaplan D. H. (1999). Nested identities, nationalism,
territory, and scale. New York: Rowman and Littlefield Publishers,
Inc.
Koos, A. K. (2007). Utilitarian explanations of support for the Euro-
pean Union. Paper presented at the midwestern political science as-
sociation’s annual conference in Chicago.
http://www.mpsanet.org/Conference/ConferencePaperArchive/tabid/
681/Default.aspx
Kra, P. (2002). The concept of national character in 18th century
France. Cromohs, 7, 1-6.
http://www.cromohs.unifi.it/7_2002/kra.html
Lijphart, A. (1999). Patterns of Democracy: Government Forms and
Performance in Thirty-Six Countries. New Haven: Yale University
Press.
Medrano, J. D., & Gutiérrez, P. (2001). Nested identities: National and
European identity in Spain. Ethnic & Racial Studies, 24, 753-780.
doi:10.1080/01419870120063963
Nikolas, M. M. (1999). False opposites in nationalism: An examination
of the dichotomy of civic nationalism and ethnic nationalism in
modern Europe.
www.nationalismproject.org/pdf/Nikolas.pdf
Özkirimli, Umut (2000). Theories of nationalism. London: Macmillan
Press.
Reicher, S., & Hopkins, N. (2001). Self and nation: Categorization,
contestation and mobilization. New York: Sage Publishers.
Risse, T. (2005). Neofunctionalism, European identity, and the puzzles
of European integration. Journal of European Public Policy, 12,
291-309.
Robyn, R. (2005). The changing face of European identity. London:
Routledge.
Smith, A. D. (1998). Nationalism and modernism: A critical survey of
recent theories of nations and nationalism. London: Routledg e.
Spiering, M. (1999). The future of national identity in the European
Union. National Identities, 1, 151-159.
doi:10.1080/14608944.1999.9728108
Tilly, C. (1990). Coercion, capital, and European states, AD 990-1990.
Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Van Evera, S. (2001). Primordialism lives! APSA-CP: Newsletter of the
Organized Section in Comparative Politics of the American Political
Science Association , 12, 20-22.
Walzer, M. (1990). What it means to be an American. Social Research,
57, 591-614.
Yashar, D. (2005). Contesting citizenship in Latin America: Indigenous
movements and the postliberal challenge. Cambridge, MA: Cam-
bridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511790966
17In this sample o f 33 countries, the trends d etected for the advanced group hold
for others as well, though the principal components are less crisp for the whole
sample.
Copyright © 2012 SciRes. 57
A. K. KOOS
Appendix
Abdelal, R., Herrera, Y. M., Johnston, A. I., & McDermott,
R. (2006). Identity as a variable. Perspectives on Politics, 4,
695-711. doi:10.1017/S1537592706060440
Anderson, B. (1983). Imagined communities: Reflections on
the origin and spread of nationalism. New York: Verso.
Bendix, R. (1964). Nation-building and citizenship: Studies
in our changing political order. New York: Wiley.
Breakwell, G. M. (2004). Identity change in the context of
the growing influence of European union institutions. In R. K.
Herrmann, T. Risse, & M. B. Brewer (Eds.), Transnational
identities: Becoming European in the EU (pp. 25-39). Lanham,
Maryland: Rowman & Littlefield.
Brewer, M. B. (2007). The importance of being we: Human
nature and intergroup relations. American Psychologist, 62,
728-738. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.62.8.728
Bruter, M. (2003). Winning hearts and minds for europe.
Comparative Political Studies, 36, 1148-1180.
doi:10.1177/0010414003257609
Bruter, M. (2004). On what citizens mean in feeling “Euro-
pean”: Perceptions of news, symbols and borderless-ness.
Journal of Ethnic & Migration Studies, 30, 21-40.
doi:10.1080/1369183032000170150
Caporaso, J. A. (2005). The possibilities of a European iden-
tity. Brown Journal of World Affairs, 12, 65-75.
Chandra, K. (2006). What is ethnic identity and does it mat-
ter? Annual Review of Political Science, 9, 397-424.
doi:10.1146/annurev.polisci.9.062404.170715
Chatterjee, P. (1993). The nation and its fragments: Colonial
and postcolonial histories. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
Press.
Checkel, J. T. (1999). Norms, institutions and national iden-
tity in contemporary Europe. International Studies Quarterly,
43, 83-114. doi:10.1111/0020-8833.00112
Delanty, G. (2005). What does it mean to be a “European”?
Innovation: The European Journal of Social Sciences, 18, 11-
22.
Gellner, E. (1983). Nations and nationalism. Oxford: Black-
well.
Gellner, E. (1997) Nationalism. New York: New York Uni-
versity Press.
Haas, E. (1986). What is Nationalism and why should we
study it? International Organization, 40, 707-744.
doi:10.1017/S0020818300027326
Herrmann, R. K., Risse , T., & Brewer, M. B. (2004). Trans-
national identities: Becoming European in the EU. New York:
Rowman & Littlefield.
Hobsbawm, E. J. (1992). Nations and nationalism since
1780: Programme, myth, reality (2nd ed.) Cambridge, MA:
Cambridge University Press.
Horowitz, D. L. (1985). Ethnic groups in conflict: Theories,
patterns, and policies. Berkeley, CA: University of California
Press.
Horowitz, D. L. (1991). A democratic south Africa? Consti-
tutional engineering in a divided society. Berkeley, CA: Uni-
versity of California Press.
Lecours, A. (2005) Structuring nationalism. In A. Lecours
(Ed.), New institutionalism: Theory and analysis. Toronto: Uni-
versity of Toronto Press.
McLaren, L. (2006). Identity, interests and attitudes to Eu-
ropean integration. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Moore, M. (2001). The ethics of nationalism. Oxford Univer-
sity Press.
Oommen, T. K. (1997). Citizenship, nationality and ethnicity:
Reconciling co mpe ti ng identities. New York: Polity Press.
Shulman, S. (2002). Nationalist sentiments and globalization
preferences in five great powers. Global Society, 16, 47-68.
doi:10.1080/09537320120111906
Copyright © 2012 SciRes.
58